Jump to ContentJump to Main Navigation
The Origins of MoralityAn Evolutionary Account$

Dennis Krebs

Print publication date: 2011

Print ISBN-13: 9780199778232

Published to Oxford Scholarship Online: September 2011

DOI: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199778232.001.0001

Show Summary Details
Page of

PRINTED FROM OXFORD SCHOLARSHIP ONLINE (www.oxfordscholarship.com). (c) Copyright Oxford University Press, 2017. All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a monograph in OSO for personal use (for details see http://www.oxfordscholarship.com/page/privacy-policy). Subscriber: null; date: 25 February 2017

(p.275) References

(p.275) References

Source:
The Origins of Morality
Publisher:
Oxford University Press

Bibliography references:

Adams, J. S. (1965). Inequity in social exchange. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 2) (pp. 266–300). New York: Academic Press.

Ainslie, G. (1975). Specious reward: A behavioral theory of impulsiveness and impulse control. Psychological Bulletin, 82, 463–496.

Alcock, J. (1998). Animal behavior: An evolutionary approach (6th ed.). Sunderland, MA: Sinauer Associates.

Alcock, J. (2001). The triumph of sociobiology. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Alexander, R. D. (1961). Aggressiveness, territoriality, and sexual behavior in field crickets. Behaviour, 17, 130–223.

Alexander, R. D. (1985). A biological interpretation of moral systems. Zygon, 20, 3–20.

Alexander, R. D. (1987). The biology of moral systems. New York: Aldine de Gruyter.

Alexander, R. D. (1990). How did humans evolve? Reflections on the uniquely unique species. Museum of Zoology, Special Publication No. 1. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press.

Alvard, M. S., & Nolin, D. A. (2002). Rousseau’s whale hunt: Coordination among big-game hunters. Current Anthropology, 43, 533–559.

Anderson, J. (1974). Bystander intervention in an assault. Paper presented at the meeting of the Southeastern Psychological Association. Hollywood, FL, May 3.

Armon, C. (1995). Moral judgment and self-reported moral events in adulthood. Journal of Adult Development, 1, 49–62.

Aronfreed, J. (1968). Conduct and conscience. New York: Academic Press.

Atran, S. (2004). In gods we trust. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Austin, W. (1980). Friendship and fairness: Effects of type of relationship and task performance on choice of distribution rules. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 6, 402–408.

Axelrod, R. (1988). The further evolution of cooperation. Science, 242, 1385–1389.

Axelrod, R., & Hamilton, W. D. (1981). The evolution of cooperation. Science, 211, 1390–1396.

Badcock, C. (1998). PsychoDarwinism: The new synthesis of Darwin and Freud. In C. Crawford & D. L. Krebs (Eds.), Handbook of evolutionary psychology (pp. 431–456). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Bandura, A. (1989). Social cognitive theory. Annals of Child Development, 6, 1–60.

Bandura, A. (1991). Social cognitive theory of moral thought and action. In W. M. Kurtines & J. L. Gewirtz (Eds.), Handbook of moral behavior and development (Vol. 1) (pp. 54–104). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Barclay, P. (2004). Trustworthiness and competitive altruism can also solve the “tragedy of the commons.” Evolution and Human Behavior, 25, 209–220.

Bargh, M.A. (1996). Automaticity in social psychology. In E. T. Higgins & A. W. Kruglanski (Eds.), Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles (Vol. 1) (pp. 1–40). New York: Guilford Press.

Barinaga, M. (1996). Social status sculpts activity of crayfish neurons. Science, 271, 290–291.

(p.276) Barrett, H. C. (2007). Modules in the flesh. In S. W. Gangestad & J. A. Simpson (Eds.), The evolution of mind: Fundamental questions and controversies (pp. 161–168). New York: Guilford Press.

Barrett, L., Dunbar, R., & Lycett, J. (2002). Human evolutionary psychology. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

Bartek, S., Krebs, D. L., & Taylor, M. (1993). Coping, defending, and the relations between moral judgment and moral behavior in prostitutes and other female juvenile delinquents. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 102, 65–73.

Batson, C. D. (1991). The altruism question: Toward a social-psychological answer. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Batson, C. D. (1998). Altruism and prosocial behavior. In D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology (4th ed.) (pp. 282–315). Boston: McGraw-Hill.

Batson, C. D. (2000). Unto others: A service . . . and a disservice. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 7, 207–210.

Batson, C. D., Batson, J. G., Griffitt, C. A., Barrientos, S., Brandt, R. J., Sprengelmeyer, P., & Bayly, M. J. (1989). Negative-state relief and the empathy-altruism hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 922–933.

Baumeister, R. F., Stillwell, A. M., & Heatherton, T. F. (1994). Guilt: An interpersonal approach. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 243–267.

Baumrind, D. (1989). Rearing competent children. In W. Damon (Ed.), Child development today and tomorrow (pp. 349–378). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

Bercovitch, F. B. (1988). Coalitions, cooperation and reproductive tactics among adult male baboons. Animal Behavior, 36, 1198–1209.

Berkowitz, L. (1972). Social norms, feelings, and other factors affecting helping behavior and altruism. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.) Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 6) (pp. 63–106). New York: Academic Press.

Bjorklund D. F., & Pellegrini, A. D. (2002). The origins of human nature: Evolutionary developmental psychology. Washington DC: American Psychological Association.

Bloom, P. (2004). Descartes’ baby: How the science of child development explains what makes us human. New York: Basic Books.

Boehm, C. (1999). Hierarchy in the forest: The evolution of egalitarian behavior. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Boehm, C. (2000). Conflict and the evolution of social control. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 7, 79–101.

Boehm, C. (2008). Purposive social selection and the evolution of human altruism. Cross-cultural Research, 24, 319–352.

Bornstein, G., & Ben-Yossef, M. (1994). Cooperation in intergroup and single-group social dilemmas. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 30, 52–67.

Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (1992). Punishment allows the evolution of cooperation (or anything else) in sizable groups. Ethology and Sociobiology, 13, 171–195.

Boyer, P. (2001). Religion explained: The human instincts that fashion Gods, spirits, and ancestors. London: William Heinemann

Brosnan, S. F., & de Waal, F. B. M. (2003). Monkeys reject unequal pay. Nature, 425, 297–299.

Brown. B. B., Lohr, M. J., & McClenahan, E. L. (1986). Early adolescents’ perceptions of peer pressure. Journal of Early Adolescence, 6, 139–154.

Brown, D. E. (1991). Human universals. New York: McGraw-Hill.

Brown, R. (1984). Social psychology: A second edition. New York: Free Press.

(p.277) Brown, S. L., & Brown, M. (2006). Selective investment theory: Recasting the functional significance of close relationships. Psychological Inquiry, 17, 30–59.

Burnham, T., & Phelan, J. (2000). Mean genes. New York: Perseus Publishing.

Burnstein, E. (2005). Altruism and genetic relatedness. In D. Buss (Ed.), The handbook of evolutionary psychology (pp. 528–551). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.

Burnstein, E., Crandall, C., & Kitayama, S. (1994). Some neo-Darwinian decision rules for altruism: Weighing cues for inclusive fitness as a function of the biological importance of the decision. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 773–789.

Burton, R. V., & Kunce, L. (1995). Behavioral models of moral development: A brief history and integration. In W. M. Kurtines & J. L. Gewirtz (Eds.), Moral development: An introduction (pp. 141–172). Boston: Allyn and Bacon.

Buss, D. (1999). Evolutionary psychology: The new science of the mind. Boston: Allyn and Bacon.

Buss. D. (2004). Evolutionary psychology: The new science of the mind (2nd ed.). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.

Buss, D. (Ed.) (2005). The handbook of evolutionary psychology. New York: John Wiley & Sons.

Buss, D. (2008) Evolutionary psychology: The new science of the mind (3rd ed). Boston: Pearson Education.

Byrne, R. W., & Whiten, A. (Eds.). (1988). Machiavellian intelligence: Social experience and the evolution of intellect in monkeys, apes, and humans. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Calvin, J. (1995). Creation of the sacred. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. (Original work published 1559).

Campbell, D. (1978). On the genetics of altruism and the counterhedonic components of human nature. In L. Wispe (Ed.). Altruism, sympathy, and helping (pp. 39–58). New York: Academic Press.

Caporael, L., Dawes, R. M., Orbell, J. M., & van de Kragt, A. J. C. (1989). Selfishness examined: Cooperation in the absence of egoistic incentives. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 12, 683–739.

Carpendale, J. I. M. (2000). Kohlberg and Piaget on stages and moral reasoning. Developmental Review, 20, 181–205.

Carpendale, J., & Krebs, D. L. (1992). Situational variation in moral judgment: In a stage or on a stage? Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 21, 203–224.

Carpendale, J., & Krebs, D. L. (1995). Variations in moral judgment as a function of type of dilemma and moral choice. Journal of Personality, 63, 289–313.

Cashdan, E. (1983). Territoriality among human foragers: Ecological models and an application to four Bushman groups. Current Anthropology, 24, 47–66.

Chadwick-Jones, J. K. (1976). Social exchange theory: Its structure and influence in social psychology. London: Academic Press.

Chagnon, N. A. (1988). Life histories, blood revenge, and warfare in a tribal population. Science, 239, 985–992.

Chaiken, S. (1987). The heuristic model of persuasion. In M.P. Zanna, J.M. Olson, & C.P. Herman (Eds.), Social influence: The Ontario Symposium (pp. 3–39). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Chaiken, S., Giner-Sorolla, R., & Chen, S. (1996). Beyond accuracy: Defense and impression motives in heuristic and systematic information processing. In P. M. Gollwitzer & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), The psychology of action: Linking cognition and motivation to behavior (pp. 553–578). New York: Guilford Press.

(p.278) Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A. (1999). The chameleon effect: The perception–behavior link and social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 893–910.

Chekroun, P., & Brauer, M. (2002). The bystander effect and social control behavior: The effect of the presence of others on people’s reaction to norm violations. European Journal of Social Psychology, 32, 853–866.

Cheney, D. L., & Seyfarth, R. M. (1990). How monkeys see the world: Inside the mind of another species. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Cialdini, R. B., Borden, R.J., Thorne, A., Walker, M. R., Freeman, S., & Sloan, L. R. (1976). Three (football) field studies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 34, 366–375.

Cialdini, R. B., Schaller, M., Houlihan, D., Arps, J., Fultz, J., & Beaman, A. L. (1987). Empathy-based helping: Is it selflessly or selfishly motivated? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 749–758.

Clark, M. S., & Mills, J. (1993). The difference between communal and exchange relationships: What it is and is not. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 19, 684–691.

Clark, M. S., Mills, J., & Powell, M. C. (1986). Keeping track of needs in communal and exchange relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 333–338.

Clark, R. D. III, & Word, L. E. (1974). Where is the apathetic bystander? Situational characteristics of the emergency. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 29, 279–287.

Colby, A., & Damon, W. (1992). Some do care. New York: Free Press.

Colby, A., & Kohlberg, L. (Eds.) (1987). The measurement of moral judgment (Vols. 1–2). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Conner, R. C., Smolker, R. A., & Richards, A. F. (1992). Dolphin alliances and coalitions. In A. H. Harcourt & F. B. M. de Waal (Eds.), Coalitions and alliances in humans and other animals (pp. 415–443). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Cosmides, L. (1989). The logic of social exchange: Has natural selection shaped how humans reason? Studies with the Wason selection task. Cognition, 31, 187–276.

Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (2005). Neurocognitive adaptations designed for social exchange. In D. Buss (Ed.), The handbook of evolutionary psychology (pp. 584–627). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.

Crawford, C. B., & Anderson, J. L. (1989). Sociobiology: An environmentalist discipline? American Psychologist, 44, 1449–1459.

Cummins, D. D. (1998). Social norms and other minds: The evolutionary roots of higher cognition. In D. D. Cummins & C. A. Allen (Eds.), The evolution of mind (pp. 30–50). New York: Oxford University Press.

Cummins, D. (2005). Dominance, status, and social hierarchies. In D. Buss (Ed.), The handbook of evolutionary psychology (pp. 676–697). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.

Daly, M., & Wilson, M. (1983). Sex, evolution, and behavior. Boston: Willard Grant Press.

Daly, M., & Wilson, M. (1988). Homicide. New York: Aldine de Gruyter.

Damasio, A. R. (1994). Decartes’ error: Emotion, reason, and the human brain. New York: Grosset/Putnam.

Damon, W. (1977). The social world of the child. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

Damon, W., & Hart, D. (1992). Self understanding and its role in social and moral development. In M. H. Bornstein & E. M. Lamb (Eds.), Developmental psychology: An advanced textbook (2nd ed.) (pp. 421–465). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Darwin, C. (1874). The descent of man and selection in relation to sex. New York: Rand, McNally & Company.

(p.279) Dawes, R., van de Kragt, A. J. C., & Orbell, J. M. (1988). Not me or thee but we: The importance of group identity in eliciting cooperation in dilemma situations: Experimental manipulations. Acta Psychologica, 68, 83–97.

Dawkins, R. (1989). The selfish gene (2nd ed.). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

DeBruine, L. M. (2002). Facial resemblance enhances trust. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B. Biological Sciences, 269, 1307–1312.

Decety, J. (2005). Perspective taking as the royal avenue to empathy. In B. F. Malle & S. D. Hodges (Eds.), Other minds: How humans bridge the divide between self and others (pp. 143–157). New York: Guilford Press.

Denton, K., & Krebs, D. L. (1990). From the scene to the crime: The effect of alcohol and social context on moral judgment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 242–248.

Denton, K., & Zarbatany, L. (1996). Age differences in support processes in conversations between friends. Child Development, 67, 1360–1137.

de Waal, F. B. M. (1982). Chimpanzee politics. London: Jonathan Cape.

de Waal, F. B. M. (1991). The chimpanzee’s sense of social regularity and its relation to the human sense of justice. American Behavioral Scientist, 34, 335–349.

de Waal, F. B. M. (1996). Good natured: The origins of right and wrong in humans and other animals. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

de Waal, F. B. M. (2006). Primates and philosophers: How morality evolved. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

de Waal, F. B. M. (2008). Putting the altruism back in altruism. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 279–300.

de Waal, F. B. M., & Brosnan, S. F. (2006). Simple and complex reciprocity in animals. In P. M. Kappeler & C. P. van Schaik (Eds.), Cooperation in primates and humans (pp. 85–105). New York: Springer-Verlag.

de Waal, F. B. M., & Luttrell, L. M. (1988). Mechanisms of social reciprocity in three primate species: Symmetrical relationship characteristics or cognition? Ethology and Sociobiology, 9, 101–118.

Dovidio, J. F., Allen, J. L., & Schroeder, D. A. (1990). The specificity of empathy-induced helping: Evidence for altruistic motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 249–260.

Dugatkin, L. A. (1997). Cooperation among animals: An evolutionary perspective. New York: Oxford University Press.

Dugatkin, L. A., & Reeve, H. K. (1994). Behavioral ecology and levels of selection: Dissolving the group selection controversy. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 23, 101–133.

Dunbar, R. I. M. (1966). Grooming, gossip, and the evolution of language. London: Faber and Faber.

Dunbar, R. I. M. (1996). Determinants of group size in primates: A general model. In G. Runciman, J. Maynard Smith, & R. I. M. Dunbar (Eds.), Evolution of social behavior patterns in primates and man (pp. 33–58). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Dunbar, R. I. M. (1998). The social brain hypothesis. Evolutionary Anthropology, 6, 187–190.

Dunbar, R. (2007). Evolution of the social brain. In S. W. Gangesad & J. A. Simpson (Eds.), The evolution of mind: Fundamental questions and controversies (pp. 280–286). New York: Guilford Press,

Duntley, J. D. (2005). Adaptations to dangers from humans. In D. Buss (Ed.), The handbook of evolutionary psychology (pp. 224–254). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.

Durham, W. H. (1991). Coevolution: Genes, culture and human diversity. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.

(p.280) Eisenberger, N. I., Lieberman, M. D., & Williams, K. D. (2003). Does rejection hurt? An fMRI study of social exclusion. Science, 302, 290–292.

Elkind, D., & Weiner, I. B. (1978). Development of the child. New York: John Wiley & Sons.

Ellis, B. J. (1998). The partner-specific investment inventory: An evolutionary approach to individual differences in investment. Journal of Personality, 66, 383–442.

Elster, J. (2000). Ulysses unbound. London: Cambridge University Press.

Essock-Vitale, S., & McGuire, M. (1985). Women’s lives viewed from an evolutionary perspective II: Patterns of helping. Ethology and Sociobiology, 6, 155–173.

Fehr, E., & Fischbacker, U. (2004). Third-party punishment and social norms. Evolution and Human Behavior, 25, 63–87.

Fehr, E., & Gächter, S. (2003). Reply. Nature, 421, 912.

Festinger, L. (1964). Conflict, decision, and dissonance. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.

Finlay, B. L. (2007). E pluribus unum: Too many unique human capacities and too many theories. In S. W. Gangesad & J. A. Simpson (Eds.), The evolution of mind: Fundamental questions and controversies (pp. 294–301). New York: Guilford Press

Fischer, P., Greitemeyer, T., Pollozek, F., & Frey, D. (2006). The unresponsive bystander: Are bystanders more responsive in dangerous emergencies? European Journal of Social Psychology, 36, 267–278.

Fischer, R. A. (1930). The genetical theory of natural selection. Oxford: Clarendon Press.

Fisher, H. (2004). Why we love: The nature and chemistry of romantic love. New York: Henry Holt.

Fiske, A. P. (1992). Four elementary forms of sociality: Framework for a unified theory of social relations. Psychological Review, 99, 689–723.

Flack, J. C., & de Waal, F. B. M. (2000). “Any animal whatever”: Darwinian building blocks of morality in monkeys and apes. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 7, 1–29.

Flack, J. C., de Waal, F. B. M., & Krakauer, D. C. (2005). Social structure robustness and policing cost in a cognitively sophisticated species. American Naturalist, 165, 126–139.

Flinn, M. V., & Alexander, R. D. (1982). Culture theory: The developing synthesis from biology. Human Ecology, 10, 383–400.

Flinn, M. V. & Coe, K. (2007). The linked red queens of human cognition, coalitions, and culture. In S. W. Gangesad & J. A. Simpson (Eds.), The evolution of mind: Fundamental questions and controversies (pp. 339–347). New York: Guilford Press.

Flinn, M. V., & Low, B. S. (1986). Resource distribution, social competition, and mating patterns in human societies. In D. I. Rubenstein & W. Wrangham (Eds.), Ecological aspects of social evolution: Birds and mammals (pp. 217–243). Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

Flynn, F. J. (2003). How much should I give and how often? The effects of generosity and frequency of favor exchange on social status and productivity. Academy of Management Journal, 46, 539–553.

Frank, R. H. (1988). Passions within reason: The strategic role of the emotions. New York: W. W. Norton.

Frank, R. H. (2001). Cooperation through emotional commitment. In R. Nesse (Ed.), Evolution and the capacity for commitment (pp. 57–76). New York: Russell Sage Foundation.

Frankena, W. K. (1973). Ethics. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

Frankena, W. K. (1980). Thinking about morality. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press.

Freud, A. (1963). The infantile instinct life. In H. Herma and G. M. Karth (Eds.), A handbook of psychoanalysis. New York: World Publishing Company.

(p.281) Freud, S. (1925). Collected papers. London: Hogarth Press.

Gächter, S., & Herrmann, B. (2006). Human cooperation from an economic perspective. In P. M. Kappeler & C. P. van Schaik (Eds.), Cooperation in primates and humans (pp. 275–302). New York: Springer-Verlag.

Gangestad, S. W., &. Simpson, J. A. (Eds.) (2007). The evolution of mind: Fundamental questions and controversies. New York: Guilford Press.

Gardner, P. (1991). Foragers’ pursuit of individual autonomy. Current Anthropology, 32, 543–558.

Gaulin, S. J. C., & McBurney, D. H. (2004). Evolutionary psychology (2nd ed.). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Pearson Prentice Hall.

Geary, D. C. (2000). Evolution and proximate expression of human paternal investment. Psychological Bulletin. 126, 55–77.

Gelfand, D. M., Hartmann, D. P., Walder, P., & Page, B. (1973). Who reports shoplifters? A field-experimental study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 23, 276–285.

Ghiselin, M. T. (1974). The economy of nature and the evolution of sex. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.

Gibbs, J. C. (2003). Moral development and reality: Beyond the theories of Kohlberg and Hoffman. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Gibbs, J. C. (2006). Should Kohlberg’s cognitive developmental approach to morality be replaced with a more pragmatic approach? Comment on Krebs and Denton (2005). Psychological Review, 113, 666–671.

Gigerenzer, G. (2000). Adaptive thinking: Rationality in the real world. New York: Oxford University Press.

Gilligan, C. (1982). In a different voice: Psychological theory and women’s development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Gilovich, T., Keltner, D., & Nisbett, R. E. (2006). Social psychology. New York: W. W. Norton.

Gintis, H. (2000). Game theory evolving: A problem-centered introduction to modeling strategic interaction. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

Gintis, H. (2007). A framework for the unification of the behavioral sciences. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30, 1–16.

Gintis, H., Bowles, S., Boyd, R., & Fehr, E. (2003). Explaining altruistic behavior in humans. Evolution and Human Behavior, 24, 153–172.

Gintis, H. Bowles, S., Boyd, R. & Fehr, E. (2008). Gene-culture coevolution and the emergence of altruistic behavior in humans. In C. Crawford & D. L. Krebs (Eds.), Foundations of evolutionary psychology (pp. 313–329). New York: Taylor & Francis.

Goffman, E. (1959). The presentation of self in everyday life. New York: Anchor Books.

Gould, S. J., (1978). Sociobiology: The art of storytelling. New Scientist, 80, 530–533.

Gouldner, A. W. (1960). The norm of reciprocity: A preliminary statement. American Sociological Review, 25, 161–178.

Graziano, W. G., Jensen-Campbell, L. A., Todd, M., & Finch, J. F. (1997). Interpersonal attraction from an evolutionary perspective: Women’s reactions to dominant and prosocial men. In J. A. Simpson & D. T. Kenrick (Eds.), Evolutionary social psychology (pp. 141–167). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Greenberg, J. (1980). A theory of indebtedness. In K. Gergen, M. S. Greenberg, & R. H. Willis (Eds.), Social exchange: Advances in theory and research. New York: Plenum Press.

Greenberg, J., & Cohen, R. L. (1982). Equity and justice in social behavior. New York: Academic Press.

(p.282) Greene, J. (2008). The secret joke of Kant’s soul. In W. Sinnott-Armstrong (Ed.), Moral psychology (Vol. 3): The neuroscience of morality: Emotion, brain disorders, and development (pp. 35–80). Cambridge MA: MIT Press.

Grueneich, R. (1982). Issues in the developmental study of how children use intention and consequence information to make moral evaluations. Child Development, 1, 29–43.

Grusec, J. (2006). The development of moral behavior and conscience from a socialization perspective. In M. Killen & J. Smetana (Eds.), Handbook of moral development (pp. 243–266). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Haan, N., Aerts, E., & Cooper, B. (1985). On moral grounds. New York: New York University Press.

Habermas, J. (1984). Interpretive social science vs. hermeneutics. In N. Haan, R. B. Bellah, P. Rabinow, & W. Sullivan (Eds.), Social science as moral inquiry. New York: Columbia Univeristy Press.

Habermas, J. (1993). Justification and application. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Hagen, E. H., & Symons, D. (2007). Natural psychology: The environment of evolutionary adaptedness and the structure of cognition. In S. W. Gangestad & J. A. Simpson (Eds.), The evolution of the mind: Fundamental questions and controversies (pp. 38–44). New York: Guilford Press.

Haidt, J. (2001). The emotional dog and its rational tail: A social intuitionist approach to moral judgment. Psychological Review, 108, 814–834.

Haidt, J. (2007). Moral psychology and the misunderstanding of religion. Edge Foundation. www.edge.org/3rd_culture/haidt07/haidt07_index.html.

Haidt, J., & Joseph, C. (2004). Intuitive ethics: How innately prepared intuitions generate culturally variable virtues. Daedalus, (Fall), 55–66.

Hamilton, W. D. (1964). The evolution of social behavior. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 7, 1–52.

Harari, H., Harari, O., & White, R. V. (1985). The reaction to rape by American male bystanders. Journal of Social Psychology, 125, 653–658.

Harms, W. (2000). The evolution of cooperation in hostile environments. In L. D. Katz (Ed.), Evolutionary origins of morality (pp. 308–312). Exeter, UK: Short Run Press.

Hart, B. L. & Hart, L. A. (1992). Reciprocal allogrooming in impala Aepyceros melampus. Animal Behavior, 44, 1073–1083.

Hartshorne, H., & May, M. A. (1928). Studies in the nature of character. New York: Macmillan.

Haslam, N. (1997). Four grammars for primate social relations. In J. A. Simpson & D. T. Kenrick (Eds.), Evolutionary social psychology (pp. 297–316). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Hauser, M. D. (2006). Moral minds: How nature designed our universal sense of right and wrong. New York: HarperCollins Publishers.

Hauser, M. D., Chen, M. K., Chen, F., & Chuang, E. (2003). Give unto others: Genetically unrelated cotton-top tamarin monkeys preferentially give food to those who altruistically give food back. Proceedings of the Royal Society, 270, 2363–2370.

Hawkes, K. (1983). Kin selection and culture. American Ethnologist, 10, 346–363.

Hawkes, K., Bliege, K., & Bird, R. (2002). Showing off, handicap signaling, and the evolution of men’s work. Evolutionary Anthropology, 11, 58–67.

Heinsohn, R., & Packer, C. (1995). Complex cooperative strategies in group-territorial African lions. Science, 269, 1260–1262.

Helwig, C. (2006). Rights, civil liberties, and democracy across cultures. In M. Killen & J. Smetana (Eds.), Handbook of moral development (pp. 185–210). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

(p.283) Henrich, J., Boyd, R., Bowles, S., Camerer, C., Fehr, E., Gintis, H., & McElreath, R. (2001). In search of homo economicus: Behavioral experiments in 15 small-scale societies. American Economic Review, 91, 73–78.

Henrich, J., Ensminger, J., McElreath, R., Barr, A., Barrett, C., Bolyanatz, A., . . . Ziker, J. (2010). Markets, religion, community size, and the evolution of fairness and punishment. Science, 19, 1480–1484.

Higgins, E. T. (1987). Self-discrepancy theory: A theory relating self and affect. Psychological Review, 94, 319–340.

Hill, K. (2002). Altruistic cooperation during foraging by the Ache, and the evolved human predisposition to cooperate. Human Nature, 13, 105–128.

Hoffman, M. (2000). Empathy and moral development: Implications for caring and justice. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Høgh-Olesen, H. (Ed.) (2010). Human morality and society: Evolutionary & comparative perspectives. Houndmills, UK: Palgrave Macmillan.

Huston, T. L., Geis, G., & Wright, R. (1976). The angry Samaritans. Psychology Today, 85, 61–64.

Huxley, T. (1893). Evolution and ethics. The second Romanes Lecture. London: Macmillan.

Isen, A. M., & Levin, P. F. (1972). Effect of feeling good on helping: Cookies and kindness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 21, 384–388.

Janicki, M. (2004). Beyond sociobiology: A kinder and gentler evolutionary view of human nature. In C. Crawford & C. Salmon (Eds.), Evolutionary psychology: Public policy and personal decisions (pp. 51–72). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Janicki, M. G., & Krebs, D. L. (1997). Evolutionary approaches to culture. In C. Crawford & D. L. Krebs (Eds.), Handbook of evolutionary psychology: Ideas, issues, and applications (pp. 163–208). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Jankowiak, W., & Diderich, M. (2000). Sibling solidarity in a polygamous community in the USA: Unpacking inclusive fitness. Evolution and Human Behavior, 21, 125–139.

Johnson, D. D. P., Stopka, P., & Knights, S. (2003). The puzzle of human cooperation. Nature, 421, 911–912.

Johnson, D. D. P., Price, M. E., & Takezawa, M. (2008). Renaissance of the individual: Reciprocity, positive assortment, and the puzzle of human cooperation. In C. Crawford & D. L. Krebs (Eds.), Foundations of evolutionary psychology (pp. 331–352). New York: Taylor & Francis.

Kagan, J., & Lamb, S. (1987). The emergence of morality in young children. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Kaplan, H. S., Gurven, M., & Lancaster, J. B. (2007). Brain evolution and the human adaptive complex: An ecological and social theory. In S. W. Gangestad & J. A. Simpson (Eds.), The evolution of mind: Fundamental questions and controversies (pp. 269–279). New York: Guilford Press.

Kaplan, H. S., & Hill, K. (1985). Food sharing among Ache foragers: Tests of explanatory hypotheses. Current Anthropology, 26, 233–245.

Kappeler, P. M., & van Schaik, C. P. (2006). Cooperation in primates and humans: Mechanisms and evolution. Berlin: Springer-Verlag.

Kenrick, D. T., Li, N. P., & Butner, J. (2006). Dynamical evolutionary psychology: How social norms emerge from individual decisions rules. In P. A. M. van Lange & A. Kruglanski (Eds.), Bridging social psychology: The benefits of transdisciplinary approaches (pp. 285–292). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Knauft, B. M. (1987). Reconstructing violence in simple human societies: Homicide among the Gebusi of New Guinea. Current Anthropology, 28, 457–500.

(p.284) Kochanska, G., & Aksan, N. (2004). Conscience in childhood: Past, present, and future. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 50, 299–310.

Kohlberg, L. (1976). Moral stages and moralization: The cognitive-developmental approach. In T. Lickona (Ed.), Moral development and behavior: Theory, research, and social issues (pp. 31–53). New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.

Kohlberg, L. (1984). Essays in moral development: The psychology of moral development (Vol. 2). New York: Harper & Row.

Kohlberg, L., & Candee, D. (1984). The relationship of moral judgment to moral action. In L. Kohlberg (Ed.), Essays in moral development (Vol. 2). The psychology of moral development (pp. 498–581). New York: Harper & Row.

Korchmaros, J. D., & Kenny, D. A. (2001). Emotional closeness as a mediator of the effect of genetic relatedness on altruism. Psychological Science, 12, 262–265.

Krebs, D. L. (1970). Altruism: An examination of the concept and a review of the literature. Psychological Bulletin, 73, 258–303.

Krebs, D. L. (1979). Piaget’s theory of moral development: A reconsideration. Human Nature, 2, 93–95.

Krebs, D. L. (2003). Fictions and facts about evolutionary approaches to human behavior: Comment on Lickliter and Honeycutt (2003). Psychological Bulletin, 129, 1–6.

Krebs, D. L. (2005). An evolutionary reconceptualization of Kohlberg’s model of moral development. In R. Burgess & K. MacDonald (Eds.), Evolutionary perspectives on human development (pp. 243–274). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.

Krebs, D. L., & Denton, K. (1997). Social illusions and self-deception: The evolution of biases in person perception. In J. A. Simpson & D. T. Kenrick (Eds.), Evolutionary social psychology (pp. 21–47). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Krebs, D. L., & Denton, K. (2005). Toward a more pragmatic approach to morality: A critical evaluation of Kohlberg’s model. Psychological Review, 112, 629–649.

Krebs, D. L., & Denton, K. (2006). Explanatory limitations of cognitive-developmental approaches to morality. Psychological Review, 113, 672–675.

Krebs, D. L., & Denton, K. (2009). Benign folie à deux: The social construction of positive illusions: Commentary on Mckay & Dennett. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 32, 525–526.

Krebs, D. L., Denton, K., & Higgins, N. (1988). On the evolution of self-knowledge and self-deception. In K. MacDonald (Ed.), Sociobiological perspectives on human behavior (pp. 103–139). New York: Springer-Verlag.

Krebs, D. L., Denton, K., Vermeulen, S. C., Carpendale, J. I., & Bush, A. (1991). The structural flexibility of moral judgment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 1012–1023.

Krebs, D. L., Denton, K., & Wark, G. (1997). The forms and functions of real-life moral decision-making. Journal of Moral Education, 20, 131–145.

Krebs, D. L., Denton, K., Wark, G., Couch, R., Racine, T. P., Krebs, D. L. (2002). Interpersonal moral conflicts between couples: Effects of type of dilemma, role, and partner’s judgments on level of moral reasoning and probability of resolution. Journal of Adult Development, 9, 307–316.

Krebs, D. L., & Janicki, M. (2004). The biological foundations of moral norms. In M. Schaller & C. Crandall (Eds.), Psychological Foundations of Culture (pp. 25–148). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Krebs, D. L., & Laird, P. (1998). Judging yourself as you judge others: Perspective-taking, moral development, and exculpation. Journal of Adult Development, 5, 1–12.

Krebs, D. L., & Van Hesteren, F. (1994). The development of altruism: Toward an integrative model. Developmental Review, 14, 103–158.

Krebs, D. L., Vermeulen, S. C., Carpendale, J. I., & Denton, K. (1991). Structural and situational influences on moral judgment: The interaction between stage and dilemma. In W. Kurtines & J. Gewirtz. (Eds.), Handbook of moral behavior and development: Theory, research, and applications (pp. 139–169). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

(p.285) Kruger, D. L. (2003). Evolution and altruism: Combining psychological mediators with naturally selected tendencies. Evolution and human behavior, 24, 118–125.

Kunda, Z. (2000). Social cognition: Making sense of people. Cambridge MA: MIT Press.

Kurland, J. A., and Gaulin, S. J. C. (2005). Cooperation and conflict among kin. In D. Buss (Ed.), The handbook of evolutionary psychology (pp. 447–482). New York: John Wiley & Sons.

Kurzban R. (2010). Grand challenges of evolutionary psychology. Frontiers in Psychology, 1 (3), 1–3. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2010.00003.

Kurzban, R. & Aktipis, C. A. (2007). On detecting the footprints of multilevel selection in humans. In S. W. Gangestad & J. A. Simpson (Eds.). The evolution of the mind: Fundamental questions and controversies (pp. 226–232). New York: Guilford Press.

Kurzban, R., DeScioli, P., & O’Brien, E. (2007). Audience effects on moralistic punishment. Evolution and Human Behavior, 28, 75–84.

Kurzban, R., & Houser, D. (2005). Experiments investigating cooperative types in humans: A complement to evolutionary theory and simulations. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the U.S.A. 102 (5), 1803–1807.

Kurzban, R., McCabe, K., Smith, V. L., & Wilson, B. J. (2001). Incremental commitment and reciprocity in a real time public goods game. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27, 1662–1673.

Lancaster, J. G., & Lancaster, C. S. (1987). The watershed: Change in parental-investment and family-formation in the course of human evolution. In J. B. Lancaster, J. Altman, A. S. Rossi, & L. R. Sherrod (Eds.), Parenting across the life span: Biosocial dimensions (pp. 187–205). New York: Aldine de Gruyter.

Lapsley, D. K. (2006). Moral stage theory. In M. Killen & J. Smetana (Eds.), Handbook of moral development (pp. 37–66). Mahwah, NJ: Laurence Erlbaum Associates.

Latané, B., & Nida, S. (1981). Ten years of research on group size and helping. Psychological Bulletin, 89, 308–324.

Latané, B., Nida, S. A., & Wilson, D. W. (1981). The effects of group size on helping behavior. In J. P. Rushton & R. M. Sorrentino (Eds.), Altruism and helping behavior: Social, personality, and developmental perspectives (pp. 287–314). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Leakey, R. E., & Lewin, R. (1977). Origins. New York: Dutton

Lerner, M. J., & Miller, D. T. (1978). Just world research and the attribution process: Looking back and ahead. Psychological Bulletin, 85, 1030–1051.

Leventhal, G. S., & Anderson, D. (1970). Self-interest and the maintenance of equity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 15, 57–62.

Lickliter, R., & Honeycutt, H. (2003). Developmental dynamics: Toward a biologically plausible evolutionary psychology. Psychological Bulletin, 129, 819–835.

Lieberman, D., & Linke, L. (2007). The effect of social category on third party punishment. Evolutionary Psychology, 5, 289–305.

Littlefield, C. H., & Rushton, J. P. (1986). When a child dies: The sociobiology of bereavement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 797–802.

Lorenz, K. Z. (1966). On aggression. London: Methuen.

Lovejoy, C. O. (1981). The origin of man. Science, 211, 341–350.

MacDonald, K. B. (1988). Sociobiology and the cognitive-developmental tradition on moral development research. In K. B. MacDonald (Ed.), Sociobiological perspectives on human development (pp. 140–167). New York: Springer-Verlag.

(p.286) MacDonald, K. B. (1997). The coherence of individual development: An evolutionary perspective on children’s internalization of values. In J. E. Grusec & L. Kuczynski (Eds.), Parenting and children’s internalization of values: A handbook of contemporary theory (pp. 362–397). New York: John Wiley & Sons.

Maynard Smith, J. (1982). Evolution and the theory of games. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

McCullough, M. E. (2008). Beyond revenge: The evolution of the forgiveness instinct. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

McCullough, M. E., Kilpatrick, S, D., Emmons, R. A., & Larson, D. B. (2001). Is gratitude a moral affect? Psychological Bulletin, 127, 249–266.

McCullough, M. E., Kimeldorf, M. B., & Cohen, A. D. (2008). An adaptation for altruism? The social causes, social effects and social evolution of gratitude. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17, 281–285.

Milgram, S. (1974). Obedience to authority. New York: Harper.

Miller, D. T. (1999). The norm of self-interest. American Psychologist, 54, 1053–1060.

Miller, G. F. (1998). The history of passion: A review of sexual selection and human evolution. In C. Crawford & D. L. Krebs (Eds.), Evolution and human behavior: Ideas, issues and applications (pp. 87–130). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Miller, G. F. (2007a). The sexual selection of moral virtues. The Quarterly Review of Biology, 82, 97–125.

Miller, G. F. (2007b). Brain evolution. In S. W. Gangesad & J. A. Simpson (Eds.), The evolution of mind: Fundamental questions and controversies (pp. 287–293). New York: Guilford Press

Miller, J. P. (2006). Insights into moral development from cultural psychology. In M. Killen and J. G. Smetana (Eds.), Handbook of moral development (pp. 375–398). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Miller, J. G., & Bersoff, D. M. (1992). Culture and moral judgment: How are conflicts between justice and interpersonal responsibilities resolved? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 541–554.

Mischel, W., Cantor, N., & Feldman, S. (1996). Principles of self-regulation: The nature of willpower and self-control. In E. T. Higgins & A. W. Kruglanski (Eds.), Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles (pp. 329–360). New York: Guilford Press.

Moll, J., di Oliveira-Sourza, R., Zahn, R., & Grafman, J. (2008). The cognitive neuroscience of moral emotions. In Sinnott-Armstrong, W. (Ed.) Moral psychology: The neuroscience of morality: Emotion, brain disorders, and development (pp. 1–18). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Moller, A. P. (2008). Sex and sexual selection. In Crawford, C., & Krebs, D. L. (Eds.), Foundations of evolutionary psychology (pp. 71–90). New York: Taylor & Francis.

Murphy, J. G. (1982). Evolution, morality, and the meaning of life. Totowa, NJ: Rowman and Littlefield.

Nesse, R. M. (2000). How selfish genes shape moral passions. In L. D. Katz (Ed.), Evolutionary origins of morality: Cross-disciplinary perspectives (pp. 227–231). Thorverton, UK: Imprint Academic.

Nesse, R. M. (Ed.) (2001). Evolution and the capacity for commitment. New York: Russell Sage Foundation.

Nowak, M. A., & Sigmund, K. (1998). Evolution of indirect reciprocity by image scoring. Nature, 393, 573–577.

Nunez, M., & Harris, P. L. (1998). Psychological and deontic concepts: Separate domains or intimate connection? Mind and Language, 13, 153–170.

(p.287) Oliner, P. M., Oliner, S. P., Baron, L., Krebs, D. L., & Smolenska, M. Z. (Eds.) (1992). Embracing the other: Philosophical, psychological, and historical perspectives on altruism. New York: New York University Press

Osherow, N. (1995). Making sense of the nonsensical: An analysis of Jonestown. In E. Aronson (Ed.), Readings about the social animal (7th ed.) (pp. 68–86). San Francisco: W. H. Freeman.

Packer, C. (1977). Reciprocal altruism in Papio anubis. Nature, 265, 441–443.

Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1986). The elaboration likelihood model of persuasion. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (pp. 123–205). New York: Academic Press.

Piaget, J. (1932). The moral judgment of the child. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.

Piliavin, J. A., Dovidio, J. F., Gaertner, S. L., & Clark, R. D. (1981). Emergency intervention. New York: Academic Press.

Piliavin, J., & Charng, H. (1990). Altruism—A review of recent theory and research. Annual Review of Sociology, 16, 27–65.

Pizarro, D. A., & Bloom, P. (2003). The intelligence of the moral intuitions: A reply to Haidt (2001). Psychological Review, 110, 193–196.

Pizarro, D. A., Uhlmann, F., & Bloom, P. (2003). Causal deviance and the attribution of moral responsibility. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 39, 653–660.

Platek, S. M., Burch, R. L., Panyavin, I. S., Wasserman, B. H., & Gallup, G. G. (2003). How much paternal resemblance is enough? Sex differences in hypothetical investment decisions but not in the detection of resemblance. Evolution and Human Behavior, 24, 81–87.

Price, M. E. (2006). Monitoring, reputation, and “greenbeard” reciprocity in a Shuar work team. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 27, 201–219.

Prinz, J. (2007). The emotional construction of morals. New York: Oxford University Press.

Pyszczynski, T., & Greenberg, J. (1987). Toward an integration of cognitive and motivational perspectives on social inference: A biased hypothesis-testing model. Advances in experimental social psychology, 20, 297–340.

Rachlin, H. (2002). Altruism and selfishness. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25, 239–296.

Rapoport, A. C. (1965). Prisoner’s dilemma. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press.

Rawls, J. (1999) A theory of justice (rev. ed.). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Reeve, K., & Sherman, P. W. (2007). Why measuring reproductive success in current populations is valuable: Moving forward by going backward. In S. W. Gangestad & J. A. Simpson (Eds.), The evolution of the mind: Fundamental questions and controversies (pp. 90–91). New York: Guilford Press.

Rest, J. F. (1983). Morality. In J. H. Flavell & E. M. Markman (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology (Vol. 3): Cognitive development (4th ed.) (pp. 556–629). New York: John Wiley & Sons.

Richards, R. J. (1982). Darwin and the biologizing of moral behavior. In W. R. Woodward & M. G. Ash (Eds.), The problematic science: Psychology in nineteenth-century thought (pp. 43–64). New York: Praeger.

Richerson, P. J., & Boyd, R. (2001). The evolution of subjective commitment: A tribal instincts hypothesis. In R. Nesse (Ed.), Evolution and the capacity for commitment (pp. 186–219). New York: Russell Sage Foundation.

Richerson, P. J., & Boyd, R. (2005). Not by genes alone: How culture transformed human evolution. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Ridley, M. (1996). The origins of virtue: Human instincts and the evolution of cooperation. New York: Viking.

Ridley, M. (2003). Nature via nurture. New York: HarperCollins.

(p.288) Robinson, P. H., & Darley, J. M. (1995). Justice, liability, and blame: Community views and the criminal law. Boulder, CO: Westview Press.

Robinson, P., Kurzban, R., & Jones, O. D. (2007). The origins of shared institutions of justice. Vanderbilt Law Review, 60, 1631–1688.

Rozin, P., Haidt, J., & McCauley, C. R. (2000). Disgust. In M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland-Jones (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (2nd ed.) (pp. 637–653). New York: Guilford Press.

Rushton, J. P. (1999). Genetic similarity theory and the nature of ethnocentrism. In K. Thienpont & R. Cliquet (Eds.), In-group/out-group behavior in modern societies: An evolutionary perspective (pp. 75–107). The Netherlands: Belgium Vlaamse Gemeenschap/CBGC.

Rushton, J. P., Russell, R. J. H., & Wells, P. A. (1984). Genetic similarity theory: Beyond kin selection Behavior Genetics, 14, 179–193.

Sachs, J. L., Mueller, U. G., Wilcox, T. P., & Bull, J. J. (2004). The evolution of cooperation. Quarterly Review of Biology, 79, 135–160.

Saltzstein, H. D., & Kasachkoff, T. (2004). Haidt’s moral intuitionist theory: A psychological and philosophical critique. Review of General Psychology, 8, 273–282.

Schachter, S., & Singer, J. E. (1962). Cognitive, social and psychological determinants of emotional state. Psychological Review, 69, 379–399.

Schaller, M. (2007). Turning garbage into gold: Evolutionary universals and cross-cultural differences. In S. W. Gangestad & J. A. Simpson (Eds.), The evolution of mind: Fundamental questions and controversies (pp. 363–369). New York: Guilford Press.

Schaller, M., & Cialdini, R. B. (1988). The economics of empathic helping: Support for a mood management motive. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 24, 163–181.

Scheel, D., & Packer, C. (1991). Group hunting behavior of lions: A search for cooperation. Animal behavior, 41, 711–722.

Scheyd, G. J., Garver-Apgar, C. E., & Gangestad, S. W. (2008). Physical attractiveness: Signals of phenotypic quality and beyond. In C. Crawford & D. L. Krebs (Eds.), Foundations of evolutionary psychology (pp. 239–260). New York: Taylor & Francis.

Schnall, S., Haidt, J., & Clore, G. L. (2005). Disgust as embodied moral judgment. Unpublished manuscript, Department of Psychology, University of Virginia.

Segal, N. L. (1984). Cooperation, competition, and altruism within twin sets: A reappraisal. Ethology and Sociobiology, 5, 163–177.

Selman, R. L. (1980). The growth of interpersonal understanding. New York: Academic Press.

Shackelford, T. K., & Buss, D. M. (1996). Betrayal in mateships, friendships, and coalitions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, 1151–1164.

Shweder, R. A., Much, N. C., Mahapatra, M., & Park, L. (1997). The “big three” of morality (autonomy, community, divinity), and the “big three” explanations of suffering. In A. Brandt & P. Rozin (Eds.), Morality and health (pp. 119–169). New York: Routledge.

Siegel, E., & Rachlin, H. (1995). Soft commitment: Self-control achieved by response persistence. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 64, 117–128.

Sime, J. D. (1983). Affiliative behavior during escape to building exits. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 3, 21–41.

Simon, H. (1990). A mechanism for social selection of successful altruism. Science, 250, 1665–1668.

Simpson, J. A., & Campbell, L. (2005). Methods of evolutionary sciences. In D. M. Buss (Ed.), The handbook of evolutionary psychology (pp. 119–144). New York: John Wiley & Sons.

Singer, P. (1981). The expanding circle: Ethics and sociobiology. New York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux.

(p.289) Skyrms, B. (2000) Game theory, rationality, and evolution of the social contract. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 7, 269–284.

Smith, M. S., Kish, B. J., & Crawford, C. B. (1987). Inheritance of wealth as human kin investment. Ethology and Sociobiology, 8, 171–182.

Snyder, C. R., & Higgins, R. L. (1988). Excuses: Their effective role in the negotiation of reality. Psychological Bulletin, 104, 23–35.

Sober, E., & Wilson, D. S. (1998). Unto others: The evolution and psychology of unselfish behavior. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Sober, E., & Wilson, D. S. (2000). Summary of Unto Others: The Evolution and Psychology of Unselfish Behavior. In L. D. Katz (Ed.), Evolutionary origins of morality: Cross disciplinary perspectives (pp. 185–206). Exeter, UK: Imprint.

Staub, E. (1979). Positive social behavior and morality. New York: Academic Press.

Strayer, F. F., & Strayer, J. (1976). An ethological analysis of social agonism and dominance relations among preschool children. Child Development, 47, 980–989.

Strayer, F. F., & Trudel, M. (1984). Developmental changes in the nature and function of social dominance among young children. Ethology and Sociobiology, 5, 279–295.

Stereley, K. (2007). An alternative evolutionary psychology? In S. W. Gangestad & J. A. Simpson (Eds.). The evolution of the mind: Fundamental questions and controversies (pp. 78–185). New York: Guilford Press.

Stevens, J. R., Cushman, F. A., & Hauser, M. D. (2005). Evolving the psychological mechanisms for cooperation. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics, 36, 499–518.

Sunstein, C. R. (2005). Moral heuristics. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28, 531–573.

Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (1985). The social identity theory of intergroup behavior. In S. Worchel & W. G. Austin (Eds.), Psychology of intergroup relations (pp. 7–24). Chicago: Nelson-Hall.

Tetlock, P. E., Visser, P. S., Singh, R. Polifroni, M., Scott, A., Elson, S. B., . . . Rescobar, P. (2007). People as intuitive prosecutors: The impact of social-control goals on attributions of responsibility. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43, 195–209.

Thaler, R. H., & Shefrin, H. M. (2005). An economic theory of self-control. In M. H. Bazerman (Ed.), Negotiation, decision making and conflict management (Vols. 1–3) (pp. 577–591). Northampton, MA: Edward Elgar Publishing.

Thompson, R., Meyer, S., & McGinley, M. (2006). Understanding values in relationships: The development of conscience. In M. Killen & J. Smetana (Eds.), Handbook of moral development (pp. 267–298). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (1992). The psychological foundations of culture. In J. Barkow, L. Cosmides, and J. Tooby (Eds.), The adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture (pp. 19–136). New York: Oxford University Press.

Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (1996). Friendship and the banker’s paradox: Other pathways to the evolution of adaptations for altruism. Proceedings of the British Academy, 88, 119–143.

Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (2005). Conceptual foundations of evolutionary psychology. In D. Buss (Ed.), The handbook of evolutionary psychology (pp. 5–67). New York: John Wiley & Sons.

Tooby, J., & Devore, I. (1987). The reconstruction of hominid behavioral evolution through strategic modeling. In W. G. Kinzey (Ed.), The evolution of human behavior: Primate models (pp. 183–237). Albany, NY: SUNY Press.

Tracy, J. L., Cheng, J. T., Robins, R. W., & Trzesniewski, K. H. (2009). Authentic and hubristic pride: The affective core of self-esteem and narcissism. Self and Identity, 8, 196–213.

(p.290) Trivers, R. L. (1971). The evolution of reciprocal altruism. Quarterly Review of Biology, 46, 35–57.

Trivers, R. (1974). Parent–offspring conflict. American Zoologist, 14, 249–264.

Trivers, R. (1985). Social evolution. Menlo Park CA: Benjamin Cummings.

Trivers, R. (2000) The elements of a scientific theory of self-deception. In D. LeCroy & P. Moller (Eds.), Evolutionary perspectives on human reproductive behavior (pp. 114–131). New York: The New York Academy of Sciences.

Trivers, R. L. (2002). Natural selection and social theory: Selected papers of Robert Trivers. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Trivers, R. (2006). Reciprocal altruism: 30 years later. In P. M. Kappeler & C. P. van Schaik (Eds.), Cooperation in primates and humans (pp. 67–84). New York: Springer-Verlag.

Turiel, E. (1983). Domains and categories in social-cognitive development. In W. Overton (Ed.), The relationship between social and cognitive development. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Turiel, E. (2006). Thought, emotions, and social interactional processes in moral development. In M. Killen & J. Smetana (Eds.), Handbook of moral development (pp. 7–36). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

van den Berghe, P. L. (1983). Human inbreeding avoidance: Culture in nature. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 6, 91–123.

Vasudev, J., and Hummel, R. C. (1987). Moral stage sequence and principled reasoning in an Indian sample. Human Development, 30, 105–118.

Walker, L. J., Pitts, R. C., Hennig, K. H., & Matsuba, M. K. (1995). Reasoning about morality and real-life moral problems. In M. Killen & D. Hart (Eds.), Morality in everyday life: Developmental perspectives (pp. 371–407). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press

Wallace, G., & Wallace, A. D. M. (Eds.) (1970). The definition of morality. London: Methuen & Co.

Wang, X. T. (2002). A kith-and-kin rationality in risky choices: Empirical examinations and theoretical modeling. In F. Salter (Ed.), Risky transactions: Trust, kinship, and ethnicity (pp. 47–70). Oxford: Berghahn.

Wark, G., & Krebs, D. L. (1996). Gender and dilemma differences in real-life moral judgment. Developmental Psychology, 32, 220–230.

Wark, G., & Krebs, D. L. (1997). Sources of variation in real-life moral judgment: Toward a model of real-life morality. Journal of Adult Development, 4, 163–178.

Webster, G. D. (2003). Prosocial behavior in families: Moderators of resource sharing. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 39, 644–652.

Wedekind, C., & Milinski, M. (2000). Cooperation through image scoring in humans. Science, 288, 850–852.

Whiten, A., & Bryne, R. W. (Eds.) (1997). Machiavellian intelligence II: Extensions and evaluations. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Wiessner, P. (2005). Norm enforcement among the Ju/hoansi Bushmen: A case of strong reciprocity? Human Nature, 16, 115–145.

Wilkinson, G. S. (1990). Food sharing in vampire bats. Scientific American, February, 76–82.

Williams, G. C. (1989). A sociobiological expansion of “Evolution and Ethics,” Evolution and Ethics (pp. 179–214). Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

Wilson, J. Q. (1993). The moral sense. New York: The Free Press.

Wrangham, R. W. (1987). African apes: The significance of African apes for reconstructing social evolution. In W. G. Kinzey (Ed.), The evolution of human behavior: Primate models. Albany: SUNY Press.

(p.291) Wright, R. (1994). The moral animal. New York: Pantheon Books.

Young, L., Bechara, A., Tranel, D., Damasio, H., Hauser, M., & Damasio, A. (2010). Damage to ventromedial prefrontal cortex impairs judgment of harmful intent. Neuron, 65, 845–851.

Young, L. Camprodon, J. A., Hauser, M., Pascual-Leone, A., & Saxe R. (2010). Disruption of the right temporo-parietal junction with transcranial magnetic stimulation reduces the role of beliefs in moral judgments. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the U.S.A., 107, 6753–6758.

Youniss, J., & Damon, W. (1992). Social construction in Piaget’s theory. In H. Beilin & P. Pufall (Eds.), Piaget’s theory: Prospects and possibilities. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Zahavi, A. (1995). Altruism as a handicap—the limitations of kin selection and reciprocity. Journal of Avian Biology, 26, 1–3.

Zahavi, A., & Zahavi, A. (1997). The handicap principle. New York: Oxford University Press.

Zajonc, R. B. (1968). Attitudinal effects of mere exposure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Monograph Supplement, 9, 2–27.

Zimbardo, P. (2005). A situationalist perspective on the psychology of good and evil: Understanding how good people are transformed into perpetrators. In A. G. Miller (Ed.), The social psychology of good and evil (pp. 21–50). New York: Guilford Press.

Zimbardo, P. G., Hane, C., Ganks, W. C., and Jaffe, D. A. (1973, April 8). Pirandellian prison: The mind is a formidable jailer. New York Times Magazine, pp. 38–60. (p.292)