Jump to ContentJump to Main Navigation
Exploring the Dynamics of Human DevelopmentAn Integrative Approach$

Catherine Raeff

Print publication date: 2016

Print ISBN-13: 9780199328413

Published to Oxford Scholarship Online: August 2016

DOI: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199328413.001.0001

Show Summary Details
Page of

PRINTED FROM OXFORD SCHOLARSHIP ONLINE (www.oxfordscholarship.com). (c) Copyright Oxford University Press, 2018. All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a monograph in OSO for personal use (for details see http://www.oxfordscholarship.com/page/privacy-policy). Subscriber: null; date: 20 June 2018

(p.235) References

(p.235) References

Source:
Exploring the Dynamics of Human Development
Publisher:
Oxford University Press

Bibliography references:

Adolph, K., & Berger, S. E. (2005). Physical and motor development. In M. H. Bornstein & M. E. Lamb (Eds.), Developmental science: An advanced textbook (pp. 223–281). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Adolph, K. E., & Robinson, S. R. (2008). In defense of change processes. Child Development, 79, 1648–1653.

Adolph, K. E., & Tamis-LeMonda, C. S. (2014). The costs and benefits of development: The transition from crawling to walking. Child Development Perspectives, 8, 187–192.

Ainsworth, M. D. S., & Bell, S. M. (1970). Attachment, exploration, and separation: Illustrated by the behavior of one-year-olds in a strange situation. Child Development, 41, 49–67.

Anisfeld, M., Turkewitz, G., Rose, S. A., Rosenberg, F. R., Sheiber, F. J., Couturier-Fagan, D. A., Ger, J. S., & Sommer, I. (2001). No compelling evidence that newborns imitate oral gestures. Infancy, 2, 111–122.

Arnett, J. J. (2000). Emerging adulthood: A theory of development from the late teens through the twenties. American Psychologist, 55, 469–480.

Azmitia, M. (1988). Peer interaction and problem solving: When are two heads better than one? Child Development, 59, 87–96.

Azmitia, M., & Montgomery, R. (1993). Friendship, transactive dialogues, and the development of scientific reasoning. Social Development, 2, 202–221.

Bachnik, J. (1992/1995). Kejime: Defining a shifting self in multiple organizational modes. In N. R. Rosenberger (Ed.), Japanese sense of self (pp. 152–172). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Bachnik, J. M. (1994). Indexing self and society in Japanese family organization. In J. M. Bachnik & C. J. Quinn, Jr. (Eds.), Situated meaning: Inside and outside in Japanese self, society, and language (pp. 143–166). Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

Baillargeon, R. (2004). Infants’ reasoning about hidden objects: Evidence for event-general and event-specific expectations. Developmental Science, 7, 391–424.

Bakeman, R., & Brownlee, J. R. (1980). The strategic use of parallel play: A sequential analysis. Child Development, 51, 873–878.

Bamberg, M. (2011). Narrative practice and identity navigation. In J. A. Holstein & J. F. Gubrium (Eds.), Varieties of narrative analysis (pp. 99–124). London: SAGE Publications.

Bamberg, M. (2012). Narrative analysis. In H. Cooper (Editor-in-chief), APA Handbook of research methods in psychology, Vol. 2 (pp. 77–94). Washington, DC: APA Press.

Bamberg, M., & Georgakopoulou, A. (2008). Small stories as a new perspective in narrative and identity analysis. Text & Talk, 28, 377–396.

Bartrip, J., Morton, J., & de Schonen, S. (2001). Responses to mother’s face in 3-week to 5-month-old infants. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 19, 212–232. (p.236)

Basseches, M., & Mascolo, M. F. (2010). Psychotherapy as a developmental process. New York: Routledge.

Bates, E., Camaioni, L., & Volterra, V. (1975). The acquisition of performatives prior to speech. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 21, 205–226.

Baumrind, D. (1967). Child care practices anteceding three patterns of preschool behavior. Genetic Psychology Monographs, 75, 43–88.

Baumrind, D. (1989). Rearing competent children. In W. Damon (Ed.), Child development today and tomorrow (pp. 349–378). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

Baumrind, D. (1996). The discipline controversy revisited. Family Relations, 45, 405–414.

Baumrind, D. (2005). Patterns of parental authority and adolescent autonomy. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 108, 61–69.

Bee, H., & Boyd, D. (2010). The developing child (12th ed.). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.

Behne, T., Carpenter, M., Gräfenhain, M., Liebal, K., Liszkowski, U., Moll, H., … Wyman, E. (2008). Cultural learning and cultural creation. In U. Müller, J. I. M. Carpendale, N. Budwig, & B. Sokol (Eds.), Social life and social knowledge: Toward a process account of development (pp. 65–101). New York: Erlbaum.

Behrens, K. Y. (2004). A multifaceted view of the concept of amae: Reconsidering the indigenous Japanese concept of relatedness. Human Development, 47, 1–27.

Bell, N. J., Wieling, E., & Watson, W. (2005). Identity development during the first two university years: Exploring intersections between micro-ontogenetic processes. New Ideas in Psychology, 23, 53–73.

Bell, R. Q., & Harper, L. V. (1977). Child effects on adults. New York: Wiley.

Bennett, M. R., & Hacker, P. M. S. (2003). Philosophical foundations of neuroscience. Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishing.

Berger, K. S. (2012). The developing person through childhood and adolescence (9th ed.). New York: Worth Publishers.

Berk, L. E. (2013). Child development (9th ed.). Boston: Pearson.

Berns, G. S., Moore, S., & Capra, C. M. (2009). Adolescent engagement in dangerous behaviors is associated with increased white matter maturity of frontal cortex. PLoS ONE 4(8): e6773, 1–12.

Bhatia, S., & Ram, A., (2009). Theorizing identity in transnational and diaspora cultures: A critical approach to acculturation. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 33, 140–149.

Bibace, R. (2005). Relating to Dr. Werner: Past and present. In J. Valsiner (Ed.), Heinz Werner and developmental science (pp. 235–258). New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers.

Billig, M. (2013). Learn to write badly: How to succeed in the social sciences. New York: Cambridge University Press.

Bjorklund, B. R. (2015). The journey of adulthood (8th ed.). Boston: Pearson.

Blum-Kulka, S., Hamo, M., & Habib, T. (2010). Explanations in naturally occurring peer talk: Conversational emergence and function, thematic scope, and contribution to the development of discursive skills. First Language, 30, 440–460.

Boker, S. M., Molenaar, P. C. M., & Nesselroade, J. R. (2009). Issues in intraindividual variability: Individual differences in equilibria and dynamics over multiple time scales. Psychology and Aging, 24, 858–862.

Bornstein, M. H. (Ed.) (2010). Handbook of cultural developmental science. New York: Psychology Press.

Bowlby, J. (1969/1982). Attachment and loss: Vol. 1: Attachment. New York: Basic Books.

Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and loss. Vol. 2: Separation: Anxiety and anger. New York: Basic Books.

Brandtstädter, J. (2006). Action perspectives on human development. In W. Damon & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology, Vol. 1 (pp. 516–568). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.

Briggs, J. L. (1998). Inuit morality play: The emotional education of a three-year-old. New Haven: Yale University Press. (p.237)

Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The ecology of human development: Experiments by nature and design. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Bronfenbrenner, U., & Morris, P. A. (2006). The bioecological model of human development. In W. Damon & R. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology, Vol. 1 (pp. 793–828). New York: Wiley.

Bronson, M. B. (2000). Self-regulation in early childhood: Nature and nurture. New York: Guilford Press.

Brownell, C. A., & Kopp, C. B. (Eds.) (2007). Socioemotional development in the toddler years: Transitions and transformations. New York: Guilford Press.

Bruner, J. (1983). Child’s talk: Learning to use language. New York: W. W. Norton.

Bruner, J. (1990) Acts of meaning. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Bruner, J. (2002). Making stories: Law, literature, life. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Bruner, J., & Lucariello, J. (1989). Monologue as narrative recreation of the world. In K. Nelson (Ed.), Narratives from the crib (pp. 73–97). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Budwig, N. (1989). The linguistic marking of agentivity and control in child language. Journal of Child Language, 16, 263–284.

Budwig, N. (2003). The role of language in human development. In J. Valsiner & K. Connolly (Eds.), Handbook of developmental psychology (pp. 217–237). London: SAGE Publications.

Bukowski, W. M., Motzoi, C., & Meyer, F. (2009). Friendship as process, function, and outcome. In K. H. Rubin, W. M. Bukowski, & B. Laursen (Eds.), Handbook of peer interactions, relationships, and groups (pp. 217–231). New York: Guilford Press.

Burman, E. (1994). Deconstructing developmental psychology. London: Routledge.

Butterworth, G. (1990). Self-perception in infancy. In D. Cicchetti & M. Beeghly (Eds.), The self in transition: Infancy to childhood (pp. 119–137). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Butterworth, G. (1998). Origins of joint visual attention in infancy. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 63, 4.

Calkins, S. D. (2007). The emergence of self-regulation: Biological and behavioral control mechanisms supporting toddler competencies. In C. A. Brownell & C. B. Kopp (Eds.). Socioemotional development in the toddler years: Transitions and transformations (pp. 261–284). New York: Guilford Press.

Calkins, S. D., & Bell, M. A. (2010). Introduction: Putting the domains of development into perspective. In S. D. Calkins & M. A. Bell (Eds.), Child development at the intersection of emotion and cognition (pp. 3–13). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Campos, J. J., Anderson, D. I., Barbu-Roth, M. A., Hubbard, E. M., Hertenstein, M. J., & Witherington, D. (2000). Travel broadens the mind. Infancy, 1, 149–219.

Cernoch, J. M., & Porter, R. H. (1985). Recognition of maternal axillary odors by infants. Child Development, 56, 1593–1598.

Chao, R. K. (1994). Beyond parental control and authoritarian parenting style: Understanding Chinese parenting through the cultural notion of training. Child Development, 65, 1111–1119.

Chen, X., Cen, G., Li, D., & He, Y. (2005). Social functioning and adjustment in Chinese children: The imprint of historical time. Child Development, 76, 182–195.

Chen, X., Chung, J., & Hsiao, C. (2009). Peer interactions and relationships from a cross-cultural perspective. In K. H. Rubin, W. M. Bukowski, & B. Laursen (Eds.), Handbook of peer interactions, relationships, and groups (pp. 432–451). New York: Guilford Press.

Chen, X., & Wang, L. (2010). China. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of cultural developmental science (pp. 429–444). New York: Psychology Press.

Chisholm, J. S. (1996). Learning “respect for everything”: Navajo images of development. In C. P. Hwang., M. E. Lamb, & I. E. Sigel (Eds.), Images of childhood (pp. 167–183). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. (p.238)

Cho, G. E., & Miller, P. J. (2004). Personal storytelling: working-class and middle-class mothers in comparative perspective. In M. Farr (Ed.), Ethnolinguistic Chicago: Language and literacy in the city’s neighborhoods (pp. 79–101). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Cole, M. (1995). The supra-individual envelope of development: Activity and practice, situation and context. New Directions for Child Development, 67, 105–118.

Cole, M. (1996). Cultural psychology: A once and future discipline. Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press/Harvard University Press.

Cook, R., Bird, G., Catmur, C., Press, C., & Heyes, C. (2014). Mirror neurons: From origin to function. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 37, 177–241.

Cooley, C. H. (1902). Human nature and the social order. New York: Scribner’s.

Coplan, R. J., & Arbeau, K. A. (2009). Peer interactions and play in early childhood. In K. H. Rubin, W. M. Bukowski, & B. Laursen (Eds.), Handbook of peer interactions, relationships, and groups (pp. 143–161). New York: Guilford Press.

Corsaro, W. A. (1986). Routines in peer culture. In J. Cook-Gumperz, W. A. Corsaro, & J. Streeck (Eds.), Children’s worlds and children’s language (pp. 231–251). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.

Corsaro, W. A. (2003). We’re friends, right”?: Inside kids’ culture. Washington, DC: Joseph Henry Press.

Corsaro, W. A., & Johannesen, B. O. (2007). The creation of new cultures in peer interaction. In J. Valsiner & A. Rosa (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of sociocultural psychology (pp. 444–459). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Damon, W., & Hart, D. (1988). Self-understanding in childhood and adolescence. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Danziger, K. (1990/1998). Constructing the subject: Historical origins of psychological research. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Danziger, K. (1997). Naming the mind: How psychology found its language. London: SAGE Publications.

DeCasper, A. J., & Fifer, W. P. (1980). Of human bonding: Newborns prefer their mothers’ voices. Science, 208, 1174–1176.

Dixon, S., Tronick, E., Keefer, C., & Brazelton, T. B. (1981). Mother-infant interaction among the Gusii of Kenya. In T. M. Field, A. M. Sostek, P. Vietze, & P. H. Leiderman (Eds.), Culture and early interactions (pp. 149–168). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Doi, T. (1973/2001). The anatomy of dependence (J. Bester, Trans.). Tokyo: Kodansha International.

Doi, T. (1974/1986). Amae: A key concept for understanding Japanese personality structure. In T. S. Lebra & W. P. Lebra (Eds.), Japanese culture and behavior: Selected readings (pp. 121–129). Honolulu: University of Hawaii Press.

Doi, T. (1985/1988). The anatomy of self: The individual versus society (M. A. Harbison, Trans.). Tokyo: Kodansha International.

Doise, W., Mugny, G., & Perret-Clermnot, A. (1975). Social interaction and the development of cognitive operations. European Journal of Social Psychology, 5, 367–383.

Downey, G., & Lende, D. H. (2012). Neuroanthropology and the encultured brain. In D. H. Lende & G. Downey (Eds.), The encultured brain: An introduction to neuroanthropology (pp. 23–65). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Dunn, J. (2004). Children’s friendships: The beginnings of intimacy. Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishing.

Dunn, J. (2008). Relationships and children’s discovery of the mind. In U. Müller, J. I. M. Carpendale, N. Budwig, & B. Sokol (Eds.), Social life and social knowledge: Toward a process account of development (pp. 171–182). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Dunn, J., & Munn, P. (1985). Becoming a family member: Family conflict and the development of social understanding in the second year. Child Development, 56, 480–492.

Durkin, K. (1995). Developmental social psychology from infancy to old age. Oxford: Blackwell. (p.239)

Duveen, G., & Psaltis, C. (2008). The constructive role of asymmetry in social interaction. In U. Müller, J. I. M. Carpendale, N. Budwig, & B. Sokol (Eds.), Social life and social knowledge: Toward a process account of development (pp. 183–204). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Dyson, J. (2014). Working childhoods: Youth, agency and the environment in India. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Edwards, C. P., de Guzman, M. R. T., Brown, J., & Kumru, A. (2006). Children’s social behaviors and peer interactions in diverse cultures. In X. Chen, D. C. French, & B. H. Schneider (Eds.), Peer relationships in cultural context (pp. 23–51). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Ehrlich, P. R. (2000). Human natures: Genes, cultures, and the human prospect. New York: Penguin Books.

Erikson, E. H. (1959/1980). Identity and the life cycle. New York: Norton.

Feldman, C. F. (1989). Monologue as problem-solving narrative. In K. Nelson (Ed.), Narratives from the crib (pp. 98–119). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Fernald, A., Marchman, V. A., & Weisleder, A. (2013). SES differences in language processing skill and vocabulary are evident at 18 months. Developmental Science, 16, 234–248.

Fischer, K. W. (1980). A theory of cognitive development: The control and construction of hierarchies of skills. Psychological Review, 87, 477–531.

Fischer, K. W., & Bidell, T. R. (2006). Dynamic development of action and thought. In W. Damon & R. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology, Vol. 1 (pp. 313–399). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.

Fischer, K., Yan, Z., & Stewart, J. (2003). Adult cognitive development: Dynamics in the developmental web. In J. Valsiner & K. Connolly (Eds.), Handbook of developmental psychology (pp. 491–516). London: SAGE Publications.

Fivush, R., Habermas, T., Waters, T. E. A., & Zaman, W. (2011). The making of autobiographical memory: Intersections of culture, narratives and identity. International Journal of Psychology, 46, 321–345.

Fivush, R., & Nelson, K. (2006). Parent-child reminiscing locates the self in the past. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 24, 235–251.

Fogel, A. (1993). Developing through relationships: Origins of communication, self, and culture. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Fogel, A. (2006). Dynamic systems research on interindividual communication: The transformation of meaning-making. Journal of Developmental Processes, 1, 7–30.

Fogel, A. (2011). Theoretical and applied dynamic systems research in developmental science. Child Development Perspectives, 5, 267–272.

Ford, D. H., & Lerner, R. M. (1992). Developmental systems theory: An integrative approach. Newbury Park, CA: SAGE Publications.

Gaskins, S. (1999). Children’s daily lives in a Mayan village: A case study of culturally constructed roles and activities. In A. Göncü (Ed.), Children’s engagement in the world: Sociocultural perspectives (pp. 25–61). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Gaskins, S. (2000). Children’s daily activities in a Mayan village: A culturally grounded description. Cross-Cultural Research: The Journal of Comparative Social Science, 34, 375–389.

Gaskins, S. (2003). From corn to cash: Change and continuity within Mayan families. Ethos, 31, 248–273.

Gaskins, S. (2006). The cultural organization of Yucatec Mayan children’s social interactions. In X. Chen, D. C. French, & B. H. Schneider (Eds.), Peer relationships in cultural context (pp. 283–309). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Gaskins, S., Haight, W., & Lancy, D. F. (2007). The cultural construction of play. In A. Göncü & S. Gaskins (Eds.), Play and development: Evolutionary, sociocultural, and functional perspectives (pp. 179–202). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Gauvain, M. (2001). The social context of cognitive development. New York: Guilford Press. (p.240)

Gazzaniga, M. S. (2011). Who’s in charge? Free will and the science of the brain. New York: Harper Collins.

Gergen, K. J. (2010). The acculturated brain. Theory and Psychology, 20, 795–816.

Gershkoff-Stowe, L., & Thelen, E. (2004). U-shaped changes in behavior: A dynamic systems perspective. Journal of Cognition and Development, 5, 11–36.

Gjerde, P. F. (2004). Culture, power, and experience: Toward a person-centered cultural psychology. Human Development, 47, 138–157.

Glick, J. A. (1983). Piaget, Vygotsky and Werner. In S. Wapner & B. Kaplan (Eds.), Toward a holistic developmental psychology (pp. 35–52). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Glick, J. A. (1992). Werner’s relevance for contemporary developmental psychology. Developmental Psychology, 28, 558–565.

Glick, M., & Zigler, E. (2005). Werner’s developmental thought in the study of adult psychopathology. In J. Valsiner (Ed.), Heinz Werner and developmental science (pp. 323–344). New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers.

Goldhaber, D. (2012). The nature-nurture debates: Bridging the gap. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Göncü, A., Mistry, J., & Mosier, C. (2000). Cultural variations in the play of toddlers. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 24, 321–329.

Goodnow, J. J., & Collins, W. A. (1990). Development according to parents: The nature, sources, and consequences of parents’ ideas. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Gottlieb, G. (1991). Experiential canalization of behavioral development: Theory. Developmental Psychology, 27, 4–13.

Gottlieb, G. (2001). The relevance of developmental-psychobiological metatheory to developmental neuropsychology. Developmental Neuropsychology, 19, 1–9.

Gottlieb, G. (2003). On making behavioral genetics truly developmental. Human Development, 46, 337–355.

Gottlieb, G. (2007). Probabilistic epigenesis. Developmental Science, 10, 1–11.

Gottlieb, G., & Halpern, C. T. (2002). A relational view of causality in normal and abnormal development. Development and Psychopathology, 14, 421–435.

Gottlieb, G., Wahlsten, D., & Lickliter, R. (1998). The significance of biology for human development: A developmental psychobiological systems view. In W. Damon (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology, Vol. 1 (pp. 233–273). New York: Wiley.

Griffiths, P. E., & Tabery, J. (2013). Developmental systems theory: What does it explain, and how does it explain it? In R. M. Lerner & J. B. Benson (Eds.), Advances in Child Development and Behavior, Vol. 44 (pp. 65–94). Amsterdam: Elsevier.

Gutiérrez, K. D., & Rogoff, B. (2003). Cultural ways of learning: Individual traits or repertoires of practice. Educational Researcher, 32, 19–25.

Habermas, T., & deSilveira, C. (2008). The development of global coherence in life narratives across adolescence: Temporal, causal, and thematic aspects. Developmental Psychology, 44, 707–721.

Haith, M. M., & Benson, J. B. (1998). Infant cognition. In W. Damon (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology, Vol. 2 (pp. 199–254). New York: Wiley.

Harkness, S., & Super, C. M. (Eds.) (1996). Parents’ cultural belief systems: Their origins, expressions, and consequences. New York: Guilford Press.

Harkness, S., Zylicz, P. O., Super, C. M., Welles-Nyström, B., Bermúdez, M. R., Bonichini, S., Moscardino, U., & Mavridis, C. J. (2011). Children’s activities and their meanings for parents: A mixed-methods study in six western cultures. Journal of Family Psychology, 25, 799–813.

Harter, S. (2012). The construction of the self: Developmental and sociocultural foundations. New York: Guilford Press.

Harwood, R. L., Miller, J. G., & Irizarry, N. L. (1995). Culture and attachment: Perceptions of the child in context. New York: Guilford Press. (p.241)

Hashmi, M. (2009). Dementia: An anthropological perspective. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 24, 207–212.

Heath, S. B. (1983/1992). Ways with words: Language, life, and work in communities and classrooms. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Heath, S. B. (1986/1992). What no bedtime story means: Narrative skills at home and school. In B. B. Schieffelin & E. Ochs (Eds.), Language socialization across cultures (pp. 97–124). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Hendry, J. (1986). Becoming Japanese: The world of the pre-school child. Honolulu: University of Hawaii Press.

Hermans, H. J. M. (2003). The construction and reconstruction of a dialogical self. Journal of Constructivist Psychology, 16, 89–130.

Herrenkohl, L. R., & Mertl, V. (2010). How students come to be, know, and do: A case for a broad view of learning. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Hickok, G. (2014). They myth of mirror neurons: The real neuroscience of communication and cognition. New York: W. W. Norton.

Ho, M. (2013). No genes for intelligence in the fluid genome. In R. M. Lerner & J. B. Benson (Eds.), Advances in child development and behavior, Vol. 45 (pp. 67–92). Amsterdam: Elsevier.

Hoff, E. (2013). Interpreting the early language trajectories of children from low-SES and language minority homes: Implications for closing achievement gaps. Developmental Psychology, 49, 4–14.

Hoffman, D. M. (2000). Pedagogies of self in American and Japanese early childhood education: A critical conceptual analysis. Elementary School Journal, 101, 193–208.

Hofstede, G. (1980/2001). Culture’s consequences: Comparing values, behaviors, institutions, and organizations across nations. Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE Publications.

Howe, M. L., & Lewis, M. D. (2005). The importance of dynamic systems approaches for understanding development. Developmental Review, 25, 247–251.

Howes, C. (1980). Peer play scale as an index of complexity of peer interaction. Developmental Psychology, 16, 371–372.

Howes, C., & Matheson, C. C. (1992). Sequences in the development of competent play with peers: Social and social pretend play. Developmental Psychology, 28, 961–974.

Howes, C., Unger, O., & Matheson, C. C. (1992). The collaborative construction of pretend: Social pretend play functions. Albany, NY: State University of New York Press.

Iverson, J. M. (2010). Developing language in a developing body: The relationship between motor development and language development. Journal of Child Language, 37, 229–261.

Jahoda, G. (2012). Critical reflections on some recent definitions of “culture.” Culture and Psychology, 18, 289–303.

James, W. (1890/1983). The principles of psychology. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Johnson, M. H. (2005). Developmental neuroscience, psychophysiology, and genetics. In M. H. Bornstein & M. E. Lamb (Eds.), Developmental science: An advanced textbook (pp. 187–222). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Johnston, T. D., & Lickliter, R. (2009). A developmental systems theory perspective on psychological change. In J. P. Spencer, M. S. C. Thomas, & J. L. McClelland (Eds.), Toward a unified theory of development: Connectionism and dynamic systems theory reconsidered (pp. 285–296). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Jones, S. S. (2009). The development of imitation in infancy. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 364, 2325–2335.

Kagan, J. (2008a). In defense of qualitative changes in development. Child Development, 79, 1606–1624.

Kagan, J. (2008b). Using the proper vocabulary. Developmental Psychobiology, 50, 4–8.

Kagan, J. (2013). The human spark: The science of human development. New York: Basic Books.

Kagan, J., & Snidman, N. (2004). The long shadow of temperament. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. (p.242)

Kaplan, B. (1967). Meditations on genesis. Human Development, 10, 65–87.

Kaplan, B. (1983a). A trio of trials. In R. M. Lerner (Ed.), Developmental psychology: Historical and philosophical perspectives (pp. 185–228). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Kaplan, B. (1983b). Genetic-dramatism: Old wine in new bottles. In S. Wapner & B. Kaplan (Eds.), Toward a holistic developmental psychology (pp. 53–74). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Kaplan, B. (1986). Value presuppositions in theories of human development. In L. Cirillo & S. Wapner (Eds.), Value presuppositions in theories of human development (pp. 89–103). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Kaplan, B. (1994). Is a concept of development applicable to art? In M. B. Franklin & B. Kaplan (Eds.), Development and the arts: Critical perspectives (pp. 3–10). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Kaplan, B., Josephs, I. E., & Bhatia, S. (2005). Re-thinking development. In J. Valsiner (Ed.), Heinz Werner and developmental science (pp. 121–154). New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers.

Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2006). The tortuous route from genes to behavior: A neuroconstructivist approach. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 6, 9–17.

Killen, M., & Wainryb, C. (2000). Independence and interdependence in diverse cultural contexts. In S. Harkness, C. Raeff, & C. M. Super (Eds.), Variability in the social construction of the child. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 87, 5–21.

Kim, Y. S., & Merriam, S. B. (2010). Situated learning and identity development in a Korean older adults’ computer classroom. Adult Educational Quarterly, 60, 438–455.

Kondo, D. K. (1990). Crafting selves: Power, gender, and discourses of identity in a Japanese workplace. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Kopp, C. B. (1982). Antecedents of self-regulation: A developmental perspective. Developmental Psychology, 18, 199–214.

Krimsky, S. (2013). Genetic causation: A cross disciplinary inquiry. In R. M. Lerner & J. B. Benson (Eds.), Advances in child development and behavior, Vol. 44 (pp. 307–323). Amsterdam: Elsevier.

Kumpulainen, K., & Kaartinen, S. (2003). The interpersonal dynamics of collaborative reasoning in peer interactive dyads. Journal of Experimental Education, 7, 333–370.

Lamiell, J. T. (2003). Beyond individual and group differences: Human individuality, scientific psychology, and William Stern’s critical personalism. Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE Publications.

Lancy, D. F. (1996). Playing on the mother-ground: Cultural routines for children’s development. New York: Guilford Press.

Lancy, D. F. (2007). Accounting for variability in mother-child play. American Anthropologist, 109, 273–284.

Lancy, D. F. (2008/2010). The anthropology of childhood: Cherubs, chattel, changelings. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Lancy, D. F. (2010). Learning ‘from nobody’: The limited role of teaching in folk models of children’s development. Childhood in the Past, 3, 79–106.

Lareau, A. (2003). Unequal childhoods: Class, race, and family life. Berkeley: University of California Press.

Laursen, B., & Pursell, G. (2009). Conflict in peer relationships. In K. H. Rubin, W. M. Bukowski, & B. Laursen (Eds.), Handbook of peer interactions, relationships, and groups (pp. 267–286). New York: Guilford Press.

Lave, J., & Wenger, E. (1991/2008). Situated learning: Legitimate peripheral participation. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Lawrence, J. A., & Valsiner, J. (2003). Making personal sense: An account of basic internalization and externalization processes. Theory and Psychology, 13, 723–752.

Lebra, T. S. (1992/1995). Self in Japanese culture. In N. R. Rosenberger (Ed.), Japanese sense of self (pp. 105–120). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Lerner, R. M. (1986). Concepts and theories of human development (2nd ed.). New York: McGraw Hill. (p.243)

Lerner, R. M. (2011). Structure and process in relational, developmental systems theories: A commentary on contemporary changes in the understanding of developmental change across the life span. Human Development, 54, 34–43.

Lerner, R. M., & Benson, J. B. (2013). Introduction: Embodiment and epigenesis: A view of the issues. Advances in Child Development and Behavior, 44, 1–19.

Letendre, G. K. (2000). Learning to be adolescent: Growing up in U.S. and Japanese middle schools. New Haven: Yale University Press.

Levine, L. W. (1977). Black culture and black consciousness: Afro-American folk thought from slavery to freedom. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

LeVine, R. A. (1990). Infant environments in psychoanalysis: A cross-cultural view. In J. W. Stigler, R. A. Shweder, & G. Herdt (Eds.), Cultural psychology: Essays on comparative human development (pp. 454–474). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

LeVine, R. A. (2004). Challenging expert knowledge: Findings from an African study of infant care and development. In U. P. Gielen & J. Roopnarine (Eds.), Childhood and adolescence: Cross-cultural perspectives and applications (pp. 149–165). Westport, CT: Praeger Publishers.

LeVine, R. A., Dixon, S., LeVine, S., Richman, A., Leiderman, P. H., Keefer, C. H., & Brazelton, T. B. (1994/1998). Child care and culture: Lessons from Africa. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

LeVine, R. A., Miller, P. M., & West, M. M. (Eds.) (1988). Parental behavior in diverse societies. New Directions for Child Development, 40.

Levitt, M. J. (2005). Social relations in childhood and adolescence: The convoy model perspective. Human Development, 48, 28–47.

Lewis, C. C. (1995/1999). Educating hearts and minds: Reflections on Japanese preschool and elementary education. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Lewis, M. (1997). Altering fate: Why the past does not predict the future. New York: Guilford Press.

Lewis, M. D. (2000). The promise of dynamic systems approaches for an integrated account of human development. Child Development, 71, 36–43.

Lewis, M. D. (2011). Dynamic systems approaches: Cool enough? Hot enough? Child Development Perspectives, 5, 279–285.

Lewontin, R. (2000). The triple helix: Gene, organism, and environment. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Lightfoot, C. (1997). The culture of adolescent risk-taking. New York: Guilford Press.

Lourenco, O., & Machado, A. (1996). In defense of Piaget’s theory: A reply to 10 common criticisms. Psychological Review, 103, 143–164.

Maccoby, E. E. (2007). Historical overview of socialization research and theory. In J. E. Grusec & P. D. Hastings (Eds.), Handbook of socialization: Theory and research (pp. 13–41). New York: The Guilford Press.

Manago, A. M. (2012). The new emerging adult in Chiapas, Mexico: Perceptions of traditional values and value change among first-generation Maya university students. Journal of Adolescent Research, 27, 663–713.

Manago, A. M., Graham, M. B., Greenfield, P. M., & Salimkhan, G. (2008). Self-presentation and gender on MySpace. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 29, 446–458.

Marshall, T. C., Chuong, K., & Aikawa, A. (2011). Day-to-day experiences of amae in Japanese romantic relationships. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 14, 26–35.

Martin, J. (2012). Coordinating with others: Outlining a pragmatic, perspectival psychology of personhood. New Ideas in Psychology, 30, 131–143.

Martin, J., & Gillespie, A. (2010). A neo-Meadian approach to human agency: Relating the social and the psychological in the ontogenesis of perspective-coordinating persons. Integrative Psychology and Behavioral Science, 44, 252–272.

Martin, J., Sugarman, J., & Thompson, J. (2003). Psychology and the question of agency. Albany, NY: State University of New York Press. (p.244)

Martini, M., & Kirkpatrick, J. (1992/1994). Parenting in Polynesia: A view from the Marquesas. In J. L. Roopnarine & D. B. Carter (Eds.), Parent-child socialization in diverse cultures (pp. 199–222). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

Mascolo, M. F. (2005). Change processes in development: The concept of coactive scaffolding. New Ideas in Psychology, 23, 185–196.

Mascolo, M. F. (2013). Developing through relationships: An embodied coactive systems framework. In R. M. Lerner & J. B. Benson (Eds.), Advances in Child Development and Behavior, Vol. 45 (pp. 185–225). Amsterdam: Elsevier.

Mascolo, M. F., & Fischer, K. W. (2007). The codevelopment of self and sociomoral emotions during the toddler years. In C. A. Brownell & C. B. Kopp (Eds.), Socioemotional development in the toddler years: Transitions and transformations (pp. 66–99). New York: Guilford Press.

Mascolo, M. F., & Fischer, K. W. (2010). The dynamic development of thinking, feeling, and acting over the life span. In R. M. Lerner (Editor-in-Chief) and W. Overton (Vol. Ed.), The handbook of life-span development, Vol. 1 (pp. 149–194). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.

Mascolo, M. F., & Fischer, K. W. (2015). The dynamic development of thinking, feeling, and acting. In R. M. Lerner, W. Overton, & P. Molenaar (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology and developmental science, Vol. 1 (pp. 113–161). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley.

Mascolo, M. F., Fischer, K. W., & Li, J. (2003). Dynamic development of component systems of emotions: Pride, shame, and guilt in China and the United States. In R. J. Davidson, K. Scherer, & H. H. Goldsmith (Eds.), Handbook of affective sciences (pp. 375–408). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

Mathews, G. (1996). What makes life worth living? How Japanese and Americans make sense of their worlds. Berkeley: University of California Press.

Maynard, A. E. (2002). Cultural teaching: The development of teaching skills in Maya sibling interactions. Child Development, 73, 969–982.

Maynard, A. E., & Tovote, K. E. (2010/2012). Learning from other children. In D. F. Lancy, J. Bock, & S. Gaskins (Eds.), The anthropology of learning in childhood (pp. 181–205). Lanham, MD: AltaMira Press.

McAdams, D. P. (1993). The stories we live by: Personal myths and the making of the self. New York: Guilford Press.

Mead, G. H. (1934/1962). Mind, self, & society. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Meadows, D. H. (2008). Thinking in systems: A primer. White River Junction, VT: Chelsea Green Publishing.

Meaney, M. J. (2010). Epigenetics and the biological definition of gene × environment interactions. Child Development, 81, 41–79.

Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1983). Newborn infants imitate adult facial gestures. Child Development, 54, 702–709.

Miller, J. G., & Kinsbourne, M. (2012). Culture and neuroscience in developmental psychology: Contributions and challenges. Child Development Perspectives, 6, 35–41.

Miller, P. J., Cho, G. E., & Bracey, J. R. (2005). Working-class children’s experience through the prism of personal storytelling. Human Development, 48, 115–135.

Miller, P. J., Fung, H., Lin, S., Chen, E. C., & Boldt, B. R. (2012). How socialization happens on the ground: Narrative practices as alternate socializing pathways in Taiwanese and European-American families. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 77, 1.

Miller, P. J., & Goodnow, J. J. (1995). Cultural practices: Toward an integration of culture and development. New Directions for Child Development, 67, 5–16.

Miller, P., & Sperry, L. L. (1987). The socialization of anger and aggression. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 33, 1–31.

Mistry, J. (2013). Integration of culture and biology in human development. In R. M. Lerner & J. B. Benson (Eds.), Advances in child development and behavior, Vol. 45 (pp. 287–314). Amsterdam: Elsevier. (p.245)

Molenaar, P. C. M. (2004). A manifesto on psychology as idiographic science: Bringing the person back into scientific psychology, this time forever. Measurement, 2, 201–218.

Molenaar, P. C. M. (2008). On the implications of the classical ergodic theorems: Analysis of developmental processes has to focus on intra-individual variation. Developmental Psychobiology, 50, 60–69.

Molenaar, P. C. M., & Campbell, C. G. (2009). The new person-specific paradigm in psychology. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18, 112–117.

Molenaar, P. C. M., & Valsiner, J. (2008). How generalization works through the single case: A simple idiographic process analysis of an individual psychotherapy. Yearbook of Idiographic Science, 1, 23–38.

Moore, C., & Povinelli, D. J. (2007). Differences in how 12- and 24-month-olds interpret the gaze of adults. Infancy, 11, 215–231.

Moshman, D. (2013). Adolescent rationality. In R. M. Lerner & J. B. Benson (Eds.), Advances in child development and behavior, Vol. 45 (pp. 155–183). Amsterdam: Elsevier.

Mosier, C. E., & Rogoff, B. (2003). Privileged treatment of toddlers: Cultural aspects of individual choice and responsibility. Developmental Psychology, 39, 1047–1060.

Nagy, E., Pilling, K., Orvos, H., & Molnar, P. (2013). Imitation of tongue protrusion in human neonates: Specificity of the response in a large sample. Developmental Psychology, 49, 1628–1638.

Nelson, D. A., Hart, C. H., Keister, E. K., & Piassetskaia, K. (2010). Russia. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of cultural developmental science (pp. 409–428). New York: Psychology Press.

Nelson, K. (Ed.) (1989). Narratives from the crib. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Nelson, K. (2007). Young minds in social worlds: Experience, meaning, and memory. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Nelson, K., & Fivush, R. (2004). The emergence of autobiographical memory: A social cultural developmental theory. Psychological Review, 11, 486–511.

Nesselroade, J. R., & Molenaar, P. C. M. (2010). Emphasizing intraindividual variability in the study of development over the life span: Concepts and issues. In R. M. Lerner (Ed.), The handbook of life-span development, Vol. 1 (pp. 30–54). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.

Nicolopoulou, A. (1997). Worldmaking and identity formation in children’s narrative play-acting. In B. D. Cox & C. Lightfoot (Eds.), Sociogenetic perspectives on internalization (pp. 157–187). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Nisbett, R. E. (2009). Intelligence and how to get it: Why schools and cultures count. New York: W. W. Norton.

Nsamenang, A. B. (1992). Human development in cultural context: A third world perspective. Newbury Park, CA: SAGE Publications.

Nsamenang, A. B. (2006). Human ontogenesis: An indigenous African view on development and intelligence. International Journal of Psychology, 41, 293–297.

Nsamenang, A. B. (2011). The culturalization of developmental trajectories: A perspective on African childhoods and adolescences. In L. A. Jensen (Ed.), Bridging cultural and developmental approaches to psychology: New syntheses in theory, research, and policy (pp. 235–254). New York: Oxford University Press.

Nsamenang, A. B., & Lo-Oh, J. L. (2010). Afrique noire. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of cultural developmental science (pp. 383–407). New York: Psychology Press.

Ochs, E., & Izquierdo, C. (2009). Responsibility in childhood: Three developmental trajectories. Ethos, 37, 391–413.

Ochs, E., & Schieffelin, B. (1984/1988). Language acquisition and socialization: Three developmental stories and their implications. In R. A. Shweder & R. A. LeVine (Eds.), Culture theory: Essays on mind, self, and emotion (pp. 276–320). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Ochs, E., & Shohet, M. (2006). The cultural structuring of mealtime socialization. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 111, 35–49. (p.246)

Odden, H., & Rochat, P. (2004). Observational learning and enculturation. Educational and Child Psychology, 21, 39–50.

Ohlsson, S. (2010). Questions, patterns, and explanations, not hypothesis testing is the core of psychology as of any science. In A. Toomela & J. Valsiner (Eds.), Methodological thinking in psychology: 60 years gone astray? (pp. 27–43). Charlotte, NC: Information Age Publishing.

Overton, W. F. (2006). Developmental psychology: Philosophy, concepts, methodology. In W. Damon & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology, Vol. 1 (pp. 18–88). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.

Overton, W. F. (2010). Life-span development: Concepts and issues. In R. M. Lerner (Ed.), Handbook of life-span development, Vol. 1 (pp. 1–29). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.

Overton, W. F. (2011). Relational developmental systems and quantitative behavior genetics: Alternative or parallel methodologies? Research in Human Development, 8, 258–263.

Overton, W. F. (2013). Relationism and relational developmental systems: A paradigm for developmental science in the post-Cartesian era. In R. M. Lerner & J. B. Benson (Eds.), Advances in child development and behavior, Vol. 4, (pp. 21–64). Amsterdam: Elsevier.

Oyama, S. (2000). Evolution’s eye: A systems view of the biology-culture divide. Durham, NC: Duke University Press.

Oyserman, D., Coon, H. M., & Kemmelmeier, M. (2002). Rethinking individualism and collectivism: Evaluation of theoretical assumptions and meta-analyses. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 3–72.

Paradise, R., & Rogoff, B. (2009). Side by side: Learning by observing and pitching in. Ethos, 37, 102–138.

Pascual-Leone, A., Amedi, A., Fregni, F., & Merabet, L. B. (2005). The plastic human brain cortex. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 28, 377–401.

Peak, L. (1991/1993). Learning to go to school in Japan: The transition from home to preschool life. Berkeley: University of California Press.

Piaget, J. (1932/1950). The moral judgment of the child (M. Gabain Trans.). London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.

Piaget, J. (1953). The origin of intelligence in the child (M. Cook, Trans.). London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.

Piaget, J. (1954/1986). The construction of reality in the child (M. Cook, Trans.). New York: Ballantine Books.

Piaget, J. (1955). The stages of intellectual development in childhood and adolescence. In H. E. Gruber & J. J. Vonèche (Eds.), The essential Piaget (pp. 814–819). New York: Basic Books.

Pipp, S. (1990). Sensorimotor and representational internal working models of self, other, and relationship: Mechanisms of connection and separation. In D. Cicchetti & M. Beeghly (Eds.), The self in transition: Infancy to childhood (pp. 243–264). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Psaltis, C., & Duveen, G. (2007). Conservation and conversation types: Forms of recognition and cognitive development. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 25, 79–102.

Rabain-Jamin, J., Maynard, A. E., & Greenfield, P. (2003). Implications of sibling caregiving for sibling relations and teaching interactions in two cultures. Ethos, 31, 204–231.

Raeff, C. (2000). European-American parents’ ideas about their toddlers’ independence and interdependence. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 21, 183–205.

Raeff, C. (2003). Patterns of cultural activity: Linking parents’ ideas and parent-child interactions. In C. Raeff & J. B. Benson (Eds.), Social and cognitive development in the context of individual, social, and cultural processes (pp. 35–53). London: Routledge.

Raeff, C. (2006a). Always separate, always connected: Independence and interdependence in cultural contexts of development. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Raeff, C. (2006b). Individuals in relation to others: Independence and interdependence in a kindergarten classroom. Ethos, 34, 521–557. (p.247)

Raeff, C. (2006c). Multiple and inseparable: Conceptualizing the development of independence and interdependence. Human Development, 49, 96–121.

Raeff, C. (2010a). Independence and interdependence in children’s developmental experiences. Child Development Perspectives, 4, 31–36.

Raeff, C. (2010b). Self constructing activities. Theory and Psychology, 20, 28–51.

Raeff, C. (2011). Distinguishing between development and change: Reviving organismic-developmental theory. Human Development, 54, 4–33.

Raeff, C. (2014). Demystifying internalization and socialization: Linking conceptions of how development happens to organismic-developmental theory. Advances in Child Behavior and Development, 46, 1–32.

Raghavan, C. S., Harkness, S., & Super, C. M. (2010). Parental ethnotheories in the context of immigration: Asian Indian immigrant and Euro-American mothers and daughters in an American town. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 41, 617–632.

Randall, W. L. (2013). Aging, irony, and wisdom: On the narrative psychology of later life. Theory and Psychology, 23, 164–183.

Ray, E., & Heyes, C. (2011). Imitation in infancy: The wealth of the stimulus. Developmental Science, 14, 92–105.

Reddy, V. (2008). How infants know minds. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Reese, E., Haden, C. A., Baker-Ward, L., Bauer, P., Fivush, R., & Ornstein, P. A. (2011). Coherence of personal narratives across the lifespan: A multidimensional model and coding method. Journal of Cognition and Development, 12, 424–462.

Ribas, R. D. C., Jr. (2010). Central and South America. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of cultural developmental science (pp. 323–339). New York: Psychology Press.

Richman, A. L., Miller, P. M., & Solomon, M. J. (1988). The socialization of infants in suburban Boston. New Directions for Child Development, 40, 65–74.

Rochat, P. (2001/2004). The infant’s world. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Rogoff, B. (1990). Apprenticeship in thinking: Cognitive development in social context. New York: Oxford University Press.

Rogoff, B. (2003). The cultural nature of human development. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

Rogoff, B., Baker-Sennett, J., Lacasa, P., & Goldsmith, D. (1995). Development through participation in sociocultural activity. New Directions for Child Development, 67, 45–65.

Rogoff, B., Mistry, J., Göncü, A., & Mosier, C. (1993). Guided participation in cultural activity by toddlers and caregivers. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 58, 8.

Rogoff, B., Moore, L., Najafi, B., Dexter, A., Correa-Chávez, M., & Solis, J. (2007). Children’s development of cultural repertoires through participation in everyday routines and practices. In J. E. Grusec & P. D. Hastings (Eds.), Handbook of socialization: Theory and research (pp. 490–515). New York: Guilford Press.

Rosenberger, N. (2001). Gambling with virtue: Japanese women and the search for self in a changing nation. Honolulu: University of Hawaii Press.

Rothbart, M. K., & Bates, J. E. (2006). Temperament. In W. Damon & R. M. Lerner, (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology, Vol. 3 (pp. 99–166). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley

Rubin, K. H., Bukowski, W. M., & Parker, J. G. (2006). Peer interactions, relationships, and groups. In W. Damon & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology, Vol. 3 (pp. 571–645). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.

Rutter, M. (2006). Genes and behavior: Nature-nurture interplay explained. Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishing.

Sacrey, L-A. R., Karl, J. M., & Whishaw, I. Q. (2012). Development of rotational movements, hand shaping, and accuracy in advance and withdrawal for the reach-to-eat movement in human infants aged 6–12 months. Infant Behavior and Development, 35, 543–560.

Salvatore, S., & Valsiner, J. (2010). Between the general and the unique: Overcoming the nomothetic versus idiographic opposition. Theory and Psychology, 20, 817–833. (p.248)

Sameroff, A. (2010). A unified theory of development: A dialectic integration of nature and nurture. Child Development, 81, 6–22.

Sawyer, R. K. (2002). Unresolved tensions in sociocultural theory: Analogies with contemporary sociological debates. Culture and Psychology, 8, 283–305.

Saxe, G. B. (2012). Cultural development of mathematical ideas: Papua New Guinea studies. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Scaife, M., & Bruner, J. S. (1975). The capacity for joint visual attention in the infant. Nature, 253, 265–266.

Schaal, B., Soussignan, R., & Marlier, L. (2002). Olfactory cognition at the start of life: The perinatal shaping of selective odor responsiveness. In B. Schaal, C. Rouby, & A. Holley (Eds.), Olfaction, taste, and cognition (pp. 421–440). West Nyack, NY: Cambridge University Press.

Schaffer, H. R. (1984). The child’s entry into a social world. London: Academic Press.

Scheper-Hughes, N. (1985). Culture, scarcity, and maternal thinking. Maternal detachment and infant survival in a Brazilian shantytown. Ethos, 13, 291–317.

Shantz, C. U., & Hartup, W. W. (1992). Conflict and development: An introduction. In C. U. Shantz & W. W. Hartup (Eds.), Conflict in child and adolescent development (pp. 1–11). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Shatz, M. (1994). A toddler’s life: Becoming a person. New York: Oxford University Press.

Shotter, J. (1975). Images of man in psychological research. London: Methuen.

Shulman, S., Feldman, B., Blatt, S. J., Cohen, O., & Mahler, A. (2005). Emerging adulthood: Age-related tasks and underlying self processes. Journal of Adolescent Research, 20, 577–603.

Shwalb, D. W., Shwalb, B. J., Nakazawa, J., Hyun, J., Le, H. V., & Satiadarma, M. P. (2010). East and Southeast Asia: Japan, South Korea, Vietnam, and Indonesia. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of cultural developmental science (pp. 445–464). New York: Psychology Press.

Shweder, R. A., Jensen, L. A., & Goldstein, W. M. (1995). Who sleeps by whom revisited: A method for extracting the moral goods implicit in practice. New Directions for Child Development, 67, 21–39.

Siegler, R. S., & Chen, Z. (2008). Differentiation and integration: Guiding principles for analyzing cognitive change. Developmental Science, 1, 433–453.

Sigel, I. E. (Ed.). (1985). Parental belief systems: The psychological consequences for children. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Sigel, I. E., McGillicuddy-DeLisi, A. V., & Goodnow, J. J. (Eds.). (1992). Parental belief systems: The psychological consequences for children. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Singer, J., Rexhaj, B., & Baddeley, J. (2007). Older, wiser, and happier? Comparing older adults’ and college students’ self-defining memories. Memory, 15, 886–898.

Smith, L. B. (2010). Action as developmental process—a commentary on Iverson’s “Developing language in a developing body: the relationship between motor development and language development.” Journal of Child Language, 37, 263–267.

Spencer, J. P., Clearfield, M., Corbetta, D., Ulrich, B., Buchanan, P., & Schöner, G. (2006). Moving toward a grand theory of development: In memory of Esther Thelen. Child Development, 77, 1521–1538.

Stern, D. (1977). The first relationship: Infant and mother. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Stern, D. N. (1985). The interpersonal world of the infant: A view from psychoanalysis and developmental psychology. New York: Basic Books.

Stiles, J. (2008). The fundamentals of brain development: Integrating nature and nurture. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Stiles, J. (2009). On genes, brains, and behavior: Why should developmental psychologists care about brain development? Child Development Perspectives, 3, 196–202.

Stipek, D., Recchia, S., & McClintic, S. (1992). Self-evaluation in young children. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 57, 1. (p.249)

Stone, J. E., Carpendale, J. I. M., Sugarman, J., & Martin, J. (2012). A Meadian account of social understanding: Taking a non-mentalistic approach to infant and verbal false belief understanding. New Ideas in Psychology, 30, 166–178.

Super, C. M., Harkness, S., Barry, O., & Zeitlin, M. (2011). Think locally, act globally: Contributions of African research to child development. Child Development Perspectives, 5, 119–125.

Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Way, N., Hughes, D., Yoshikawa, H., Kalman, R. K., & Niwa, E. Y. (2008). Parents’ goals for children: The dynamic coexistence of individualism and collectivism in cultures and individuals. Social Development, 17, 183–209.

Teasley, S. D. (1995). The role of talk in children’s peer collaboration. Developmental Psychology, 31, 207–220.

Thelen, E., & Smith, L. B. (1994/1996). A dynamic systems approach to the development of cognition and action. Cambridge, MA: Bradford Books/MIT Press.

Thelen, E., & Smith, L. B. (2006). Dynamic systems theories. In W. Damon & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology, Vol. 1 (pp. 258–312). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.

Thompson, R. A. (1999). Early attachment and later development. In J. Cassidy & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications (pp. 265–286). New York: Guilford Press.

Tobin, J. (1992/1995). Japanese preschools and the pedagogy of selfhood. In N. R. Rosenberger (Ed.), Japanese sense of self (pp. 21–39). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Tobin, J. J, Wu, D. Y. H., & Davidson, D. H. (1989). Preschool in three cultures: Japan, China, and the United States. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.

Todeschini, M. M. (2011). “Webs of engagement”: Managerial responsibility in a Japanese company. Journal of Business Ethics, 101, 45–59.

Tomasello, M. (2003). Constructing a language: A usage-based theory of language acquisition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., & Liszkowski, U. (2007). A new look at infant pointing. Child Development, 78, 705–722.

Toomela, A., & Valsiner, J. (2010). Methodological thinking in psychology: 60 years gone astray? (pp. 27–43). Charlotte, NC: Information Age Publishing.

Trevarthen, C. (1982). The primary motives for cooperative understanding. In G. Butterworth & P. Light (Eds.), Social cognition: Studies of the development of understanding (pp. 77–109). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Trevarthen, C. (1993/1996). The self born in intersubjectivity: The psychology of an infant communicating. In U. Neisser (Ed.), The perceived self: Ecological and interpersonal sources of self-knowledge (pp. 121–173). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Trevarthen, C., & Hubley, P. (1978). Secondary intersubjectivity: Confidence, confiding and acts of meaning. In A. Lock (Ed.), Action, gesture and symbol (pp. 183–229). London: Academic Press.

Tudge, J. R. H. (1992). Processes and consequences of peer collaboration: A Vygotskian analysis. Child Development, 63, 1364–1379.

Turiel, E. (1983/1985). The development of social knowledge: Morality and convention. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Turiel, E. (2003). Resistance and subversion in everyday life. Journal of Moral Education, 32, 115–130.

Turiel, E. (2008). Social decisions, social interactions, and the coordination of diverse judgments. In U. Müller, J. I. M. Carpendale, N. Budwig, & B. Sokol (Eds.), Social life and social knowledge: Toward a process account of development (pp. 255–276). New York: Erlbaum.

Turiel, E., & Perkins, S. A. (2004). Flexibilities of mind: Conflict and culture. Human Development, 47, 158–178.

Turiel, E., & Wainryb, C. (2000). Social life in cultures: Judgments, conflict, and subversion. Child Development, 71, 250–256. (p.250)

Underwood, K. (2010). Interactive remembering: Insights into the communicative competence of older adults. Journal of Aging Studies, 24, 145–166.

Užgiris, I. Č. (1977). Plasticity and structure: The role of experience in infancy. In I. Č. Užgiris & F. Weizmann (Eds.), The structuring of experience (pp. 89–113). New York: Plenum Press.

Užgiris, I. Č. (1989). Infants in relation: Performers, pupils, and partners. In W. Damon (Ed.), Child development today and tomorrow (pp. 288–311). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

Užgiris, I. Č. (1991). The social context of infant imitation. In M. Lewis & S. Feinman (Eds.), Social influences and socialization in infancy (pp. 215–251). New York: Plenum Press.

Užgiris, I. Č. (1996). Together and apart: The enactment of values in infancy. In E. S. Reed, E. Turiel, & T. Brown (Eds.), Values and knowledge (pp. 17–39). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Užgiris, I. Č. (1999). Imitation as activity: Its developmental aspects. In J. Nadel & G. Butterworth (Eds.), Imitation in infancy (pp. 186–206). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Užgiris, I. Č., Benson, J. B., Kruper, J. C., & Vasek, M. E. (1989). Contextual influences on imitative interactions between mothers and infants. In J. J. Lockman & N. L. Hazen (Eds.), Action in social context: Perspectives on early development (pp. 103–127). New York: Plenum Press.

Užgiris, I. Č., & Hunt, J. McV. (1975/1989). Assessment in infancy: Ordinal scales of psychological development. Urbana: University of Illinois Press.

Valsiner, J. (1997). Culture and the development of children’s action: A theory of human development. New York: Wiley.

Valsiner, J. (1998). The guided mind: A sociogenetic approach to personality. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Valsiner, J. (Ed.). (2005). Heinz Werner and developmental science. New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers.

Valsiner, J. (2011). The development of individual purposes: Creating actuality through novelty. In L. A. Jensen (Ed.), Bridging cultural and developmental approaches to psychology: New syntheses in theory, research, and policy (pp. 212–232). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

van Geert, P. (1998). We almost had a great future behind us: The contribution of non-linear dynamics to developmental-science-in-the-making. Developmental Science, 1, 143–159.

van Geert, P. (2003). Dynamic systems approaches and modeling of developmental processes. In J. Valsiner & K. Connolly (Eds.), Handbook of developmental psychology (pp. 640–672). London: SAGE Publications.

van Geert, P. (2011). The contribution of complex dynamic systems to development. Child Development Perspectives, 5, 273–278.

van Geert, P., & Fischer, K. W. (2009). Dynamic systems and the quest for individual-based models of change and development. In J. P. Spencer, M. S. C. Thomas, & J. L. McClelland (Eds.), Toward a unified theory of development: Connectionism and dynamic systems theory re-considered (pp. 313–336). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

van Geert, P., & Steenbeek, H. (2005). Explaining after by before: Basic aspects of a dynamic systems approach to the study of development. Developmental Review, 25, 408–442.

van Geert, P., & van Dijk, M. (2002). Focus on variability: New tools to study intra-individual variability in developmental data. Infant Behavior and Development, 25, 340–374.

von Bertalanffy, L. (1969/1998). General system theory: Foundations, development, applications. New York: George Braziller.

von Bertalanffy, L. (1972). The history and status of general systems theory. The Academy of Management Journal, 15, 407–426.

Vreeke, G. J. (2000). Nature, nurture and the future of the analysis of variance. Human Development, 43, 32–45.

Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Vygotsky, L. S. (1986/1987). Thought and language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. (p.251)

Wahlsten, D. (2003). Genetics and the development of brain and behavior. In J. Valsiner & K. Connolly (Eds.), Handbook of developmental psychology (pp. 18–47). London: SAGE Publications.

Wahlsten, D. (2013). A contemporary view of genes and behavior: Complex systems and interactions. In R. M. Lerner & J. B. Benson (Eds.), Advances in child development and behavior, Vol. 44 (pp. 285–306). Amsterdam: Elsevier.

Wapner, S. (2000). Person-in-environment transitions: Developmental analysis. Journal of Adult Development, 7, 7–22.

Wapner, S., & Demick, J. (1998). Developmental analysis: A holistic, developmental, systems-oriented perspective. In W. Damon (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology, Vol. 1 (pp. 761–805). New York: Wiley.

Wapner, S., & Demick, J. (2005). Critical person-in-environment transitions across the life span. In J. Valsiner (Ed.), Heinz Werner and developmental science (pp. 285–305). New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers.

Waterman, A. S. (1981). Individualism and interdependence. American Psychologist, 36, 762–773.

Watkins, M. (2000/2005). Invisible guests: The development of imaginal dialogues. Putnam, CT: Spring Publications.

Watson-Gegeo, K. A., & Gegeo, D. W. (1986/1992). Calling-out and repeating routines in Kwara’ae children’s language socialization. In B. B. Schieffelin & E. Ochs (Eds.), Language socialization across cultures (pp. 17–50). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Weisner, T. S., & Gallimore, R. (1977). My brother’s keeper: Child and sibling caretaking. Current Anthropology, 18, 169–190.

Wentzel, K. R. (2009). Peers and academic functioning at school. In K. H. Rubin, W. M. Bukowski, & B. Laursen (Eds.), Handbook of peer interactions, relationships, and groups (pp. 531–547). New York: Guilford Press.

Werner, H. (1940/1980). Comparative psychology of mental development. New York: International Universities Press.

Werner, H. (1957). The concept of development from a comparative and organismic point of view. In D. B. Harris (Ed.), The concept of development: An issue in the study of human behavior (pp. 125–148). Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.

Werner, H., & Kaplan, B. (1956/1978). The developmental approach to cognition: Its relevance to the psychological interpretation of anthropological and ethnolinguistic data. Reprinted in S. S. Barten & M. B. Franklin (Eds.), Developmental processes: Heinz Werner’s selected writings, Vol. 1 (pp. 85–106). New York: International Universities Press.

Werner, H., & Kaplan, B. (1963/1984). Symbol formation: An organismic-developmental approach to the psychology of language. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Wertsch, J. V. (1985). Vygotsky and the social formation of mind. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Wertsch, J. V. (1991). Voices of the mind: A sociocultural approach to mediated action. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Wertsch, J. V. (1998). Mind as action. New York: Oxford University Press.

West, E. (1989). Growing up with the country: Childhood on the far Western frontier. Albuquerque: University of New Mexico Press.

Wexler, B. E. (2006). Brain and culture: Neurobiology, ideology, and social change. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

White, M. (1987). The Japanese educational challenge: A commitment to children. New York: The Free Press.

Whiting, B. (2004). Women as agents of social change. In C. P. Edwards & B. B. Whiting (Eds.), Ngecha: A Kenyan village in a time of rapid social change (pp. 93–117). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.

Whiting, B., Chesaina, C., Diru, G., Ichoya, J., Kariuki, P., Kimani, V. N., … Streeter, L. (2004). Changing concepts of the good child and good mothering. In C. P. Edwards & (p.252) B. B. Whiting (Eds.), Ngecha: A Kenyan village in a time of rapid social change (pp. 119–151). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.

Whiting, B. B., & Edwards, C. P. (1988). Children of different worlds: The formation of social behavior. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Williams, A. L., & Merten, M. J. (2008). A review of online social networking profiles by adolescents: Implications for future research and intervention. Adolescence, 43, 253–274.

Witherington, D. C. (2007). The dynamic systems approach as metatheory for developmental psychology. Human Development, 50, 127–153.

Witherington, D. C. (2011). Taking emergence seriously: The centrality of circular causality for dynamic systems approaches to development. Human Development, 54, 66–92.

Witherington, D. C., & Heying, S. (2013). Embodiment and agency: Toward a holistic synthesis for developmental science. In R. M. Lerner & J. B. Benson (Eds.), Advances in child development and behavior, Vol. 44 (pp. 161–192). Amsterdam: Elsevier.

Wood, D., Bruner, J. S., & Ross, G. (1976). The role of tutoring in problem-solving. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 17, 89–100.

Xu, Y., & Farver, J. A. M. (2009). “What makes you shy?”: Understanding situational elicitors of shyness in Chinese children. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 33, 97–104.

Yamaguchi, S., & Ariizumi, Y. (2006). Close interpersonal relationships among Japanese: Amae as distinguished from attachment and dependence. In U. Kim, K. Yang, & K. Hwang (Eds.), Indigenous and cultural psychology: Understanding people in context (pp. 163–174). New York: Springer.

Yan, Z., & Fischer, K. (2002). Always under construction: Dynamic variations in adult cognitive microdevelopment. Human Development, 45, 141–160.

Yan, Z., & Fischer, K. (2007). Pattern emergence and pattern transition in microdevelopmental variation: Evidence of complex dynamics of developmental processes. Journal of Developmental Processes, 2, 39–62.

Youniss, J. (2008). Making common perspectives collaboratively: The flesh and blood of everyday social life. Human Development, 51, 332–335.

Zukow-Goldring, P. (2002). Sibling caregiving. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of parenting, Vol. 3 (pp. 253–286). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.