Jump to ContentJump to Main Navigation
Doing without Concepts$

Edouard Machery

Print publication date: 2009

Print ISBN-13: 9780195306880

Published to Oxford Scholarship Online: May 2009

DOI: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195306880.001.0001

Show Summary Details
Page of

PRINTED FROM OXFORD SCHOLARSHIP ONLINE (www.oxfordscholarship.com). (c) Copyright Oxford University Press, 2017. All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a monograph in OSO for personal use (for details see http://www.oxfordscholarship.com/page/privacy-policy). Subscriber: null; date: 24 February 2017

(p.253) References

(p.253) References

Source:
Doing without Concepts
Publisher:
Oxford University Press

Bibliography references:

Abbott, B. 1997. A note on the nature of water. Mind 106:311–319.

—— 1999. Water = H2O. Mind 108:145–148.

Ahn, W. K. 1998. Why are different features central for natural kinds and artifacts? The role of causal status in determining feature weights. Cognition 69:135–178.

Ahn, W. K., and Luhmann, C. C. 2004. Demystifying theory‐based categorization. In Building object categories in developmental time, ed. L. Gershkoff‐Stowe and D. Rakison, 277–300. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Ahn, W. K., Kim, N. S., Lassaline, M. E., and Dennis, M. J. 2000. Causal status as a determinant of feature centrality. Cognitive Psychology 41:361–416.

Ahn, W. K., Gelman, S. A., Amsterlaw, J. A., Hohenstein, J., and Kalish, C. W. 2000. Causal status effect in children's categorization. Cognition 76:B35–B43.

Allen, S. W., and Brooks, L. R. 1991. Specializing the operation of an explicit rule. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 120:3–19.

Anderson, J. R. 1978. Arguments concerning representations for mental imagery. Psychological Review 85:249–277.

Anderson, J. R., and Betz, J. 2001. A hybrid model of categorization. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 8:629–647.

Armstrong, S. L., Gleitman, L. R., and Gleitman, H. 1983. What some concepts might not be. Cognition 13:263–308.

Ashby, F. G., and Ell, S. W. 2001. The neurobiology of category learning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 5:204–210.

—— 2002. Single versus multiple systems of learning and memory. In Stevens' handbook of experimental psychology. Vol. 4, Methodology in experimental psychology. 3d ed., ed. J. Wixted and H. Pashler, 655–692. New York: Wiley.

(p.254) Ashby, F. G., and Ennis, J. M. 2006. The role of the basal ganglia in category learning. In The psychology of learning and motivation. Vol. 46, ed. B. H. Ross, 1–36. New York: Elsevier.

Ashby, F. G., and Gott, R. E. 1988. Decision rules in the perception and categorization of multidimensional stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 14:33–53.

Ashby, F. G., and Maddox, W. T. 1990. Integrating information from separable psychological dimensions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 16:598–612.

—— 1993. Relations between exemplar, prototype, and decision bound models of categorization. Journal of Mathematical Psychology 37:372–400.

—— 2005. Human category learning. Annual Review of Psychology 56:149–178.

Ashby, F. G., and O'Brien, J. B. 2005. Category learning and multiple memory systems. Trends in Cognitive Science 2:83–89.

Ashby, F. G., and Valentin, V. V. 2005. Multiple systems of perceptual category learning: Theory and cognitive tests. In Categorization in cognitive science, ed. H. Cohen and C. Lefebvre, 548–573. New York: Elsevier.

Ashby, F. G., and Waldron, E. M. 2000. The neuropsychological bases of category learning. Current Directions in Psychological Science 9:10–14.

Ashby, F. G., Alfonso‐Reese, L. A., Turken, A. U., and Waldron, E. M. 1998. A neuropsychological theory of multiple systems in category learning. Psychological Review 105:442–481.

Baddeley, A. 1986. Working memory. Oxford: Clarendon Press.

Bailenson, J. B., Shum, M. S., Atran, S., Medin, D., and Coley, J. D. 2002. A bird's eye view: Biological categorization and reasoning within and across cultures. Cognition 84:1–53.

Baillargeon, R., Kotovsky, L., and Needham, A. 1995. The acquisition of physical knowledge in infancy. In Causal cognition: A multidisciplinary debate, ed. D. Sperber, D. Premack, and A. J. Premack, 79–116. New York: Oxford University Press.

Barclay, J. R., Bransford, J. D., Franks, J. J., McCarrell, N. S., and Nitsch, K. E. 1974. Comprehension and semantic flexibility. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 13:471–481.

Barsalou, L. W. 1983. Ad hoc categories. Memory & Cognition 10:82–93.

—— 1985. Ideals, central tendency, and frequency of instantiation as determinants of graded structure in categories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 11:629–654.

—— 1987. The instability of graded structure: Implications for the nature of concepts. In Concepts and conceptual development: Ecological and intellectual factors in categorization, ed. U. Neisser, 101–140. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

—— 1989. Intraconcept similarity and its implications for interconcept similarity. In Similarity and analogical reasoning, ed. S. Vosniadou and A. Ortony, 76–121. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

—— 1990. On the indistinguishability of exemplar memory and abstraction in category representation. In Advances in social cognition. Vol. 3, Content and process specificity in the effects of prior experiences, ed. T. K. Srull and R. S. Wyer, 61–88. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

—— 1993. Flexibility, structure, and linguistic vagary in concepts: Manifestations of a compositional system of perceptual symbols. In Theories of memory, ed. (p.255) A. C. Collins, S. E. Gathercole, and M. A. Conway, 29–101. London: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

—— 1999. Perceptual symbol systems. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22:577–660.

—— 2003. Abstraction in perceptual symbol systems. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London: Biological Sciences 358:1177–1187.

—— 2008. Grounded cognition. Annual Review of Psychology 59:617–645.

Barsalou, L. W., Solomon, K. O., and Wu, L. L. 1999. Perceptual simulation in conceptual tasks. In Cultural, typological, and psychological in cognitive linguistics: The proceedings of the 4th conference of the International Cognitive Linguistics Association, ed. M. K. Hiraga, C. Sinha, and S. Wilcox, 209–228. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

Barsalou, L. W., Simmons, W. K., Barbey, A., and Wilson, C. D. 2003. Grounding conceptual knowledge in modality‐specific systems. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 7:84–91.

Barsalou, L. W., Pecher, D., Zeelenberg, R., Simmons, W. K., and Hamann, S. B. 2005. Multi‐modal simulation in conceptual processing. In Categorization inside and outside the lab: Essays in honor of Douglas L. Medin, ed. W. Ahn, R. Goldstone, B. Love, A. Markman, and P. Wolff, 249–270. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Batson, C. D. 1991. The altruism question: Toward a social‐psychological answer. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Beer, J. S., Shimamura, A. P., and Knight, R. T. 2004. Frontal lobe contributions to executive control of cognitive and social behavior. In The Cognitive Neurosciences. 3d ed., ed. M. S. Gazzaniga, 1091–1104. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Berkeley, G. [1734] 1998. A treatise concerning the principles of human knowledge, ed. J. Dancy. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Berretty, P. M., Todd, P. M., and Martignon, L. 1999. Categorization by elimination. In Simple heuristics that make us smart, ed. G. Gigerenzer, P. M. Todd, and the ABC Research Group, 235–254. New York: Oxford University Press.

Binet, A. 1903. L'Étude expérimentale de l'intelligence. Paris: Schleicher Frères et Cie.

Bishop, M. A., and Stich, S. P. 1998. The flight to reference, or how not to make progress in the philosophy of science. Philosophy of Science 65:33–49.

Bloom, P. 1996. Intention, history, and artifact concepts. Cognition 60:1–29.

—— 2000. How children learn the meanings of words. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Boghossian, P. 1996. Analyticity reconsidered. Nous 30:360–391.

Boring, E. G. 1950. A history of experimental psychology. New York: Appleton‐Century‐Crofts.

Bornkeneau, P. 1990. Traits as ideal‐based and goal‐derived social categories. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 58:381–396.

Boyd, R. 1990. What realism implies and what it does not. Dialectica 43:5–29.

—— 1991. Realism, anti‐foundationalism and the enthusiasm for natural kinds. Philosophical Studies 61:127–148.

—— 1999. Kinds, complexity and multiple realization. Philosophical Studies 95:67–98.

Boyd, R., and Silk, J. A. 2000. How humans evolved. New York: W. W. Norton.

Boyer, P., and Barrett, H. C. 2005. Domain specificity and intuitive ontology. In Handbook of evolutionary psychology, ed. D. M. Buss, 200–223. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.

(p.256) Brigandt, I. 2005. A theory of conceptual advance. Ph.D. diss., University of Pittsburgh.

Brooks, L. R. 1978. Nonanalytic concept formation and memory for instances. In Cognition and concepts, ed. E. Rosch and B. B. Lloyd, 169–211. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Brooks, L. R., Norman, G. R., and Allen, S. W. 1991. The role of specific similarity in a medical diagnostic task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 120:278–287.

Brooks, R. A. [1991] 1999. Intelligence without representation. Artificial Intelligence 47:139–159. Reproduced in R. A. Brooks, Cambrian Intelligence. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Bruner, J. S., Goodnow, J.‐J., and Austin, G. A. 1956. A study of thinking. New York: Wiley.

Burge, T. 1979. Individualism and the mental. Midwest Studies in Philosophy 4:7–121.

Busemeyer, J., and Townsend, J. T. 1993. Decision field theory: A dynamic‐cognitive approach to decision making. Psychological Review 100:432–459.

Cabeza, R., Bruce., V., Kato, T., and Oda, M. 1999. The prototype effect in face recognition: Extension and limits. Memory & Cognition 27:139–151.

Cantor, N., and Mischel, W. 1979. Prototypes in person perception. In Advances in experimental social psychology. Vol. 12, ed. L. Berkowitz, 3–52. New York: Academic Press.

Cantor, N., Mischel, W., and Schwartz, J. C. 1982. A prototype analysis of psychological situations. Cognitive Psychology 14:45–77.

Cantor, N., Smith, E. E., French, R., and Mezzich, J. 1980. Psychiatric diagnosis as prototype categorization. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 89:181–193.

Caramazza, A. 1986. On drawing inferences about the structure of normal cognitive systems from the analysis of patterns of impaired performance: The case for single‐patient studies. Brain & Cognition 5:41–66.

Caramazza, A., and Mahon, B. Z. 2003. The organization of conceptual knowledge: Evidence from category‐specific semantic deficits. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 7:354–361.

—— 2006. The organization of conceptual knowledge in the brain: The future's past and some future directions. Cognitive Neuropsychology 23:13–38.

Carey, S. E. 1985. Conceptual change in childhood. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

—— 1988. Conceptual differences between children and adults. Mind & Language 3:167–181.

—— 1991. Knowledge acquisition: enrichment or conceptual change? In The epigenesis of mind: Essays in biology and cognition, ed. S. E. Carey and R. Gelman, 257–291. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Carey, S. E., and Bartlett, E. 1978. Acquiring a single new word. Proceedings of the Stanford Child Language Conference 15:17–29.

Carey, S. E., and Johnson, S. C. 2000. Metarepresentations and conceptual change: Evidence from Williams syndrome. In Metarepresentation: A multidisciplinary perspective, ed. D. Sperber, 225–264. New York: Oxford University Press.

Carey, S. E., and Spelke, E. S. 1994. Domain specific knowledge and conceptual change. In Mapping the mind: Domain specificity in cognition and culture, ed. L. A. Hirschfeld and S. A. Gelman, 169–200. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

(p.257) Chaiken, S., and Trope, Y. 1999. Dual‐process theories in social psychology. New York: Guilford Press.

Chalmers, D. 2006. Two‐dimensional semantics. In Oxford handbook of the philosophy of language, ed. E. Lepore and B. Smith, 574–606. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Chaplin, W. F., John, O. P., and Goldberg, L. R. 1988. Conceptions of states and traits: Dimensional attributes with ideals as prototypes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 54:541–557.

Cheng, P. W., and Holyoak, K. J. 1985. Pragmatic versus syntactic approaches to training deductive reasoning. Cognitive Psychology 17:391–416.

Chomsky, N. 1995. Language and nature. Mind 104:1–61.

Churchland, P. M. 1981. Eliminative materialism and the propositional attitudes. Journal of Philosophy 78:67–90.

Clark, A. 1993. Associative engines: Connectionism, concepts, and representational change. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Coleman, L., and Kay, P. 1981. Prototype semantics: The English word lie. Language 57:26–44.

Collier, J. 1996. On the necessity of natural kinds. In Natural kinds, laws of nature and scientific reasoning, ed. P. Riggs, 1–10. Dordrecht: Kluwer.

Coltheart, M., and Davies, M. 2003. Inference and explanation in cognitive neuropsychology. Cortex 39:188–191.

Coltheart, M., Curtis, B., Atkins, P., and Haller, M. 1993. Models of reading aloud: Dual‐route and parallel‐distributed‐processing approaches. Psychological Review 100:589–608.

Coltheart, M., Rastle, K., Perry, C., Langdon, R., and Ziegler, J. 2001. DRC: A dual route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. Psychological Review 108:204–256.

Conant, M. B., and Trabasso, T. 1964. Conjunctive and disjunctive concept formation under equal‐information conditions. Journal of Experimental Psychology 67:250–255.

Connolly, A. C., Fodor, J. A., Gleitman, L. R., and Gleitman, H. 2007. Why stereotypes don't even make good defaults. Cognition 103:1–22.

Cosmides, L. 1989. The logic of social exchange: Has natural selection shaped how humans reason? Studies with the Wason selection task. Cognition 31:187–276.

Costello, F. J., and Keane, M. T. 2000. Efficient creativity: Constraint‐guided conceptual combination. Cognitive Science 24:299–349.

—— 2005. Compositionality and the pragmatics of conceptual combination. In The compositionality of meaning and content. Vol. 2, Applications to linguistics, psychology and neuroscience, ed. E. Machery, M. Werning, and G. Schultz, 203–216. Frankfurt: Ontos.

Craver, C. 2004. Dissociable realization and kind splitting. Philosophy of Science 71:960–971.

Damasio, A. R. 1989. Time‐locked multiregional retroactivation: A systems‐level proposal for the neural substrates of recall and recognition. Cognition 33:25–62.

—— 1994. Descartes' error: Emotion, reason, and the human brain. New York: Grosset/Putnam.

Danks, D. 2007. Theory unification and graphical models in human categorization. In Causal learning: Psychology, philosophy, and computation, ed. A. Gopnik and L. Schulz, 173–189. New York: Oxford University Press.

(p.258) Davies, R. R., Hodges, J. R., Kril, J. J., Patterson, K., Halliday, G. M., and Xuereb, J. H. 2005. The pathological basis of semantic dementia. Brain 128:1984–1995.

Dennett, D. C. 1969. Content and consciousness. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.

—— 1993. Learning and labeling. Mind & Language 8:540–548.

—— 1996. Kinds of minds: Toward an understanding of consciousness. New York: Basic Books.

Devlin, J. T., Rushworth, M. F. S., and Matthews, P. M. 2005. Category‐related activation for written words in the posterior fusiform is task specific. Neuropsychologia 43:69–74.

Dias, M., and Harris, P. L. 1990. The influence of the imagination on reasoning by young children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology 8:305–318.

Dunn, J. C. 2003. The elusive dissociation. Cortex 39:177–179.

Dunn, J. C., and Kirsner, K. 1988. Discovering functionally independent mental processes: The principle of reversed association. Psychological Review 95:91–101.

—— 2003. What can we infer from double dissociations? Cortex 39:1–7.

Estes, W. K. 1986. Array models of concept learning. Cognitive Psychology 18:500–549.

Evans, J. St. B. T., and Over, D. E. 1996. Rationality and reasoning. Hove: Psychology Press.

Farah, M. J. 2004. Visual cognition. 2d ed. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Faucher, L., Mallon, R., Nichols, S., Nazer, D., Ruby, A., Stich, S. P., and Weinberg, J. 2002. The baby in the labcoat: Why child development is an inadequate model for understanding the development of science. In The cognitive basis of science, ed. P. Carruthers, S. P. Stich, and M. Siegal, 335–362. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Feeney, A., and Heit, E., eds. 2007. Inductive reasoning: Experimental, developmental, and computational approaches. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Felleman, D. J., and Van Essen, D. C. 1991. Distributed hierarchical processing in primate cerebral cortex. Cerebral Cortex 1:1–47.

Filoteo, J. V., Todd Maddox, W., Simmons, A. N., Ing, A. D., Cagigas, X. E., Matthews, S., and Paulus, M. P. 2005. Cortical and subcortical brain regions involved in rule‐based category learning. Neuroreport 16:111–115.

Fisher, S. C. 1916. The process of generalizing abstraction; and its product, the general concept. Psychological Monographs, 21:1–209.

Fodor, J. A. 1974. Special sciences. Synthese 28:97–115.

—— 1975. The language of thought. New York: Crowell.

—— 1981. The present status of the innateness controversy. In Representations: Philosophical essays on the foundations of cognitive science, by J. A. Fodor, 257–316. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

—— 1994. Concepts: A potboiler. Cognition 50:95–113.

—— 1998. Concepts: Where cognitive science went wrong. New York: Oxford University Press.

Fodor, J. A., and Pylyshyn, Z. W. 1988. Connectionism and cognitive architecture: A critical analysis. Cognition 28:3–71.

Fodor, J. A., Garret, M. F., Walker, E. C. T., and Parkes, C. H. 1980. Against definitions. Cognition 8:263–367.

Gärdenfors, P. 2000. Conceptual spaces: The geometry of thought. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

(p.259) Gelman, R. 1990. First principles organize attention to and learning about relevant data: Number and the animate–inanimate distinction as examples. Cognitive Science 14:79–106.

—— 2004. Cognitive development. In Stevens' handbook of experimental psychology. Vol. 3, Memory and cognitive processes, ed. H. Pashler and D. L. Medin, 533–560. New York: Wiley.

Gelman, S. A. 1988. The development of induction within natural kinds and artifacts categories. Cognitive Psychology 20:65–95.

—— 2003. The essential child: Origins of essentialism in everyday thought. New York: Oxford University Press.

Gelman, S. A., and Bloom, P. 2000. Young children are sensitive to how an object was created when deciding what to name it. Cognition 76:91–103.

—— 2007. Developmental changes in the understanding of generics. Cognition 105:163–183.

Gelman, S. A., and Coley, J. D. 1991. Language and categorization: The acquisition of natural kind terms. In Perspectives on language and thought: Interrelations in development, ed. S. A. Gelman and J. P. Byrnes, 146–196. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Gelman, S. A., and Markman, E. 1986. Categories and induction in young children. Cognition 23:183–209.

—— 1987. Young children's inductions from natural kinds: The role of categories and appearances. Child Development 58:1532–1541.

Gelman, S. A., and Medin, D. L. 1993. What's so essential about essentialism? Different perspective on the interaction of perception, language, and conceptual knowledge. Cognitive Development 8:157–167.

Gelman, S. A., and Wellman, H. M. 1991. Insides and essences: Early understandings of the non‐obvious. Cognition 38:213–244.

Gengerelli, J. A. 1927. Mutual interference in the evolution of concepts. American Journal of Psychology 38:639–646.

Gennari, S. P., Sloman, S., Malt, B., and Fitch, T. 2002. Motion events in language and cognition. Cognition 83:49–79.

Gigerenzer, G., and Goldstein, D. G. 1996. Reasoning the fast and frugal way: Models of bounded rationality. Psychological Review 103:650–669.

Gigerenzer, G., and Regier, T. P. 1996. How do we tell an association from a rule? Psychological Bulletin 119:23–26.

Gigerenzer, G., Todd, P. M., and the ABC Research Group. 1999. Simple heuristics that make us smart. New York: Oxford University Press.

Gleitman, L. R., Connolly, A. C., and Armstrong, S. L. Forthcoming. Can prototype representations support composition and decomposition over features and words? In The Oxford handbook of compositionality, ed. M. Werning, W. Hinzen, and E. Machery. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Glenberg, A. M. 1997. What memory is for? Behavioral and Brain Sciences 20:1–55.

Glymour, C. 1994. On the methods of cognitive neuropsychology. British Journal for the Philosophy of Science 45: 815–835.

—— 2001. The mind's arrows: Bayes nets and graphical causal models. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Goldman, A. I. 2006. Simulating minds: The philosophy, psychology, and neuroscience of mindreading. New York: Oxford University Press.

Goldstein, D. G., and Gigerenzer, G. 2002. Models of ecological rationality: The recognition heuristic. Psychological Review 109:75–90.

(p.260) Goldstone, R. L., and Kersten, A. W. 2003. Concepts and categorization. In Handbook of psychology, ed. I. B. Weiner. Vol. 4, Experimental psychology, ed. A. F. Healy and R. W. Proctor, 599–621. New York: Wiley.

Gopnik, A., and Meltzoff, A. N. 1997. Words, thoughts, and theories. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Gopnik, A., and Schulz, L. 2004. Mechanisms of theory‐formation in young children. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 8:371–377.

—— eds. 2007. Causal learning: Psychology, philosophy, and computation. New York: Oxford University Press.

Gopnik, A., and Wellman, H. M. 1994. The theory theory. In Mapping the mind: Domain specificity in cognition and culture, ed. L. A. Hirschfeld and S. A. Gelman, 257–293. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Gopnik, A., Glymour, C., Sobel, D., Schulz, L., Kushnir, T., and Danks, D. 2004. A theory of causal learning in children: Causal maps and Bayes nets. Psychological Review 111:1–31.

Goschke, T. 1997. Implicit learning and unconscious knowledge: Mental representations, computational mechanisms and brain structure. In Knowledge, concepts, and categories, ed. K. Lamberts and D. Shanks, 247–334. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Grant, C. M., Riggs, K. J., and Boucher, J. 2004. Counterfactual and mental state reasoning in children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 34:177–188.

Greene, J. D., and Haidt, J. 2002. How and where does moral judgment work? Trends in Cognitive Sciences 6:517–523.

Greene, J. D., Nystrom, L. E., Engell, A. D., Darley, J. M., Cohen, J. D. 2004. The neural bases of cognitive conflict and control in moral judgment. Neuron 44:389–400.

Greene, J. D., Sommerville, R. B., Nystrom, L. E., Darley, J. M., and Cohen, J. D. 2001. An fMRI investigation of emotional engagement in moral judgment. Science 293 (Sept. 14):2105–2108.

Greene, J. D., Morelli, S. A., Lowenberg, K., Nystrom, L. E., and Cohen, J. D. 2008. Cognitive load selectively interferes with utilitarian moral judgment. Cognition 107:44–54.

Griffiths, P. E. 1997. What emotions really are. Chicago: Chicago University Press.

Griffiths, T. L., and Tenenbaum, J. B. 2007. Two proposals for causal grammar. In Causal learning: Psychology, philosophy, and computation, ed. A. Gopnik and L. Schulz, 323–346. New York: Oxford University Press.

Griffiths, T. L., Steyvers, M., and Tenenbaum, J. B. 2007. Topics in semantic representation. Psychological Review 114:211–244.

Grossman, M., Koenig, P., DeVita, C., Glosser, G., Alsop, D., Detre, J., and Gee, J. 2002. The neural basis for category‐specific knowledge: An fMRI study. Neuroimage 15:936–948.

Grünbaum, A. 1908. Über die Abstracktion der Gleichheit. Archiv für die Geschichte der Psychologie 7:340–478.

Grush, R. 2003. In defense of some “Cartesian” assumptions concerning the brain and its operation. Biology & Philosophy 18:53–93.

—— 2004. The emulation theory of representation: Motor control, imagery, and perception. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 27:377–396.

(p.261) Hacking, I. 1986. Making up people. In Reconstructing individualism: Autonomy, individuality, and the self in western thought, ed. T. Heller, M. Sosna, and D. Wellberry, 222–236. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.

—— 1991. A tradition of natural kinds. Philosophical Studies 61:109–126.

—— 1999. The social construction of what? Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Hadjichristidis, C., Sloman, S. A., Stevenson, R. J., and Over, D. E. 2004. Feature centrality and property induction. Cognitive Science 28:45–74.

Hahn, U., and Chater, N. 1998. Similarity and rules: Distinct? exhaustive? empirically distinguishable? Cognition 65:197–230.

Hahn, U., and Ramscar, M., eds. 2001. Similarity and Categorization. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Haidt, J. 2001. The emotional dog and its rational tail: A social intuitionist approach to moral judgment. Psychological Review 108:814–834.

Hammond, K. 1989. Case‐based planning: Viewing planning as a memory task. New York: Academic Press.

Hampton, J. A. 1979. Polymorphous concepts in semantic memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 18:441–461.

—— 1981. An investigation of the nature of abstract concepts. Memory & Cognition 9:149–156.

—— 1982. A demonstration of intransitivity in natural categories. Cognition 12:151–164.

—— 1987. Inheritance of attributes in natural concept conjunctions. Memory & Cognition 15:55–71.

—— 1988. Overextension of conjunctive concepts: Evidence for a unitary model of concept typicality and class inclusion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition?14:378–383.

—— 1993. Prototype models of concept representation. In Categories and concepts: Theoretical views and inductive data analysis, ed. I. Van Mechelen, J. A., Hampton, R. S. Michalski, and P. Theuns, 67–95. London: Academic Press.

—— 1995. Testing the prototypes theory of concepts. Journal of Memory and Language 34:686–708.

—— 1996. Conjunctions of visually‐based categories: Overextensions and compensation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 34:686–708.

—— 1997a. Conceptual combination. In Knowledge, concepts, and categories, ed. K. Lamberts and D. Shanks, 133–160. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

—— 1997b. Conceptual combination: Conjunction and negation of natural concepts. Memory & Cognition 25:888–909.

—— 1997c. Psychological representation of concepts. In Cognitive models of memory, ed. M. A. Conway, 81–110. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

—— 1998. Similarity‐based categorization and fuzzyness of natural categories. Cognition 65:137–165.

—— 2001. The role of similarity in natural categorization. In Similarity and categorization, ed. U. Hahn and M. Ramscar, 13–28. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

—— 2006. Concepts as prototypes. In The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory. Vol. 46, ed. B. H. Ross, 79–113. New York: Academic Press.

(p.262) Hampton, J. A. 2007. Typicality, graded membership, and vagueness. Cognitive Science 31:355–384.

Hampton, J. A., and Dubois, D. 1993. Psychological models of concepts: Introduction. In Categories and concepts: Theoretical views and inductive data analysis, ed. I. Van Mechelen, J. Hampton, R. S. Michalski, and P. Theuns, 11–33. London: Academic Press.

Hampton, J. A., and Jönsson, M. L. Forthcoming. Typicality and compositionality: The logic of combining vague concepts. In The Oxford handbook of compositionality, ed. M. Werning, W. Hinzen, and E. Machery. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Hare, B., Brown, B., Williamson, C., and Tomasello, M. 2002. The domestication of social cognition in dogs. Science 298:1634–1636.

Heit, E. 1997. Knowledge and concept learning. In Knowledge, concepts, and categories, ed. K. Lamberts and D. Shanks, 7–42. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

—— 2000. Properties of inductive reasoning. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 7:569–592.

—— 2001. Background knowledge and models of categorization. In Similarity and categorization, ed. U. Hahn and M. Ramscar, 155–178. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Heit, E., and Rubinstein, J. 1994. Similarity and property effects in inductive reasoning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 20:411–422.

Hempel, C. G., and Oppenheim, P. 1948. Studies in the logic of explanation. Philosophy of Science 15:135–175.

Hewson, C. 1994. Empirical evidence regarding the folk psychological concept of belief. In Proceedings of the sixteenth annual conference of the Cognitive Science Society, ed. A. Ram and K. Eiselt, 403–406. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Hintzman, D. L. 1986. Schema abstraction in a multiple‐trace memory model. Psychological Review 93:411–428.

Hirschfeld, L. A., and Gelman, S. A., eds. 1994. Mapping the mind: Domain‐specificity in cognition and culture. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Homa, D., Sterling, S., and Trepel, L. 1981. Limitations of exemplar‐based generalization and the abstraction of categorical information. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory 7:418–439.

Horn, B. K. P. 1975. Obtaining shape from shading information. In The psychology of computer vision, ed. P. H. Winston, 115–155. New York: McGraw‐Hill.

Hull, C. L. 1920. Quantitative aspects of the evolution of concepts. Psychological Monographs, 28:1–86.

Hull, D. 1978. A matter of individuality. Philosophy of Science 45:335–360.

Hume, D. [1748] 1975. Enquiries concerning human understanding and concerning the principles of morals. Ed. P. H. Nidditch. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Huttenlocher, J., and Hedges, L. V. 1994. Combining graded categories: Membership and typicality. Psychological Review 101:157–165.

Inagaki, K., and Hatano, G. 2006. Young children's conceptions of the biological world. Current Directions in Psychological Science 15:177–181.

Inhelder, B., and Piaget, J. 1969. The early growth of logic in the child: Classification and seriation. New York: W. W. Norton.

Jackendoff, R. 1992. What is a concept, that a person may grasp it? In Languages of the mind: Essays on mental representation, by R. Jackendoff, 21–52. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

(p.263) James, W. [1890] 1950. The principles of psychology. New York: Dover Publications.

Johnson, C., and Keil, F. C. 2000. Explanatory understanding and conceptual combination. In Explanation and cognition, ed. F. C. Keil and R. A. Wilson, 327–359. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Jones, G. V. 1983. Identifying basic categories. Psychological Bulletin 94:423–428.

Jones, S. S., and Smith, L. B. 1993. The place of perception in children's concepts. Cognitive Development 8:113–139.

Jönsson, M. L., and Hampton, J. A. 2007. On prototypes as defaults. Cognition 106:913–923.

Juslin, P., and Persson, M. 2002. PROBabilities from EXemplars: A “lazy” algorithm for probabilistic inference from generic knowledge. Cognitive science 26:133–156.

Kahneman, D., and Frederick, S. 2002. Representativeness revisited: Attribute substitution in intuitive judgment. In Heuristics & biases: The psychology of intuitive judgment, ed. T. Gilovich, D. Griffin, and D. Kahneman, 49–81. New York: Cambridge University Press.

Kamp, H., and Partee, B. 1995. Prototype theory and compositionality. Cognition 57:129–191.

Kan, I. P., Barsalou, L. W., Solomon, K. O., Minor, J. K., and Thompson‐Schill, S. L. 2003. Role of mental imagery in a property verification task: fMRI evidence for perceptual representations of conceptual knowledge. Cognitive Neuropsychology 20:525–540.

Katz, J. J., and Fodor, J. A. 1963. The structure of a semantic theory. Language 39:170–210.

Keil, F. C. 1987. Category structure and patterns of conceptual change. In Concepts and conceptual development: The ecological and intellectual factors in categorization, ed. U. Neisser, 175–200. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

—— 1989. Concepts, kinds, and cognitive development. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

—— 1991. Theories, concepts, and word meaning. In Perspectives on language and thought: Interrelations in development, ed. S. A. Gelman and J. P. Byrnes, 197–224. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

—— 2003. Folkscience: Coarse interpretations of a complex reality. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 7:368–373.

Keil, F. C., and Wilson, R. A. eds. 2000. Explanation and cognition. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Keil, F. C., Carter Smith, W., Simons, D. J., and Levin, D. T. 1998. Two dogmas of conceptual empiricism: Implications for hybrid models of the structure of knowledge. Cognition 65:103–135.

Keil, F. C., Levin, D., Gutheil, G., and Richman, B. 1999. Explanation, cause and mechanism: The case of contagion. In Folkbiology, ed. D. Medin and S. Atran, 285–320. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Kemp, C. S., and Tenenbaum, J. B. 2003.Theory‐based induction. In Proceedings of the twenty-fifth annual conference of the Cognitive Science Society, ed. R. Alterman and D. Kirsh, 658–663. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Kiefer, M., Sim, E.‐J., Liebich, S., Hauk, O., and Tanaka, J. 2007. Experience‐dependent plasticity of conceptual representations in human sensory—motor areas. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 19:525–542.

Kim, J. 1992. Multiple realization and the metaphysics of reduction. Philosophy and Phenomenological Research 52:1–26.

(p.264) Knowlton, B. J. 1997. Declarative and nondeclarative knowledge: Insights from cognitive neuroscience. In Knowledge, concepts, and categories, ed. K. Lamberts and D. R. Shanks, 214–246. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

—— 1999. What can neuropsychology tell us about category learning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 3:123–124.

Knowlton, B. J., and Squire, L. R. 1993. The learning of categories: Parallel brain systems for item memory and category knowledge. Science 262: 1747–1749.

Komatsu, L. K. 1992. Recent views of conceptual structure. Psychological Bulletin 112:500–526.

Kripke, S. A. [1972] 1980 Naming and necessity. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Kruschke, J. K. 1992. ALCOVE: An exemplar‐based connectionist model of category learning. Psychological Review 99:22–44.

Kunda, Z., Miller, D. T., and Claire, T. 1990. Combining social concepts: The role of causal reasoning. Cognitive Science 14:551–577.

Kuo, Z. Y. 1923. A behavioristic experiment on inductive inference. Journal of Experimental Psychology 6:247–293.

Labov, W. 1973. The boundaries of words and their meanings. In New ways of analyzing variation in English, ed. C. J. Baily and R. Shuy, 340–373. Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press.

Lakoff, G., and Johnson, M. 1999. Philosophy in the flesh: The embodied mind and its challenge to western thought. New York: Basic Books.

Lancaster, J. S., and Barsalou, L. W. 1997. Multiple organisations of events in memory. Memory 5:569–599.

Laporte, J. 1998. Living water. Mind 107:451–455.

Laurence, S., and Margolis, E. 1999. Concepts and cognitive science. In Concepts: Core readings, ed. E. Margolis and S. Laurence, 3–82. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

LeDoux, J. E. 1996. The emotional brain. New York: Simon and Schuster.

Leevers, H. J., and Harris, P. L. 2000. Counterfactual syllogistic reasoning in normal four‐year‐olds, children with learning disabilities, and children with autism. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 76:64–87.

Locke, J. [1690] 1979. An essay concerning human understanding. Ed. P. H. Nidditch. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Logan, G. D. 1988. Toward an instance theory of automaticity. Psychological Review 95:492–527.

López, A., Gelman, S. A., Gutheil, G., and Smith, E. E. 1992. The development of category based induction. Child Development 63:1070–1090.

López, A., Atran, S., Coley, J. D., Medin, D. L., and Smith E. E. 1997. The tree of life: Universal and cultural features of folkbiological taxonomies and inductions. Cognitive Psychology 32:251–295.

Luhmann, C. C., Ahn, W. K., and Palmeri, T. 2006. Theory‐based categorization under speeded conditions. Memory & Cognition 34:1102–1111.

Lynch, E. B., Coley, J. D., and Medin, D. L. 2000. Tall is typical: Central tendency, ideal dimensions and graded category structure among tree experts and novices. Memory & Cognition 28:41–50.

Machamer, P., Darden, L., and Craver, C. 2000. Thinking about mechanisms. Philosophy of Science 67:1–25.

Machery, E. 2005. Concepts are not a natural kind. Philosophy of Science 72:444–467.

(p.265) —— 2006a. How to split concepts: Reply to Piccinini and Scott. Philosophy of Science 73:410–418.

—— 2006b. Two dogmas of neo‐empiricism. Philosophy Compass 1:398–412.

—— 2007a. Concept empiricism: A methodological critique. Cognition 104: 19–46.

—— 2007b. 100 Years of psychology of concepts: The theoretical notion of concept and its operationalization. Studies in History and Philosophy of Biological and Biomedical Sciences 38:63–84.

Machery, E., and Barrett, C. 2006. Debunking Adapting Minds. Philosophy of Science 73:232–246.

Machery, E., and Lederer, L. G. Forthcoming. Simple heuristics for concept composition. In The Oxford handbook of compositionality, ed. M. Werning, W. Hinzen, and E. Machery. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Machery, E., and Seppälä, S. 2008. Against hybrid theories of concepts. Manuscript.

Machery, E., Mallon, R., Nichols, S., and Stich, S. P. 2004. Semantics, cross‐cultural style. Cognition 92:B1–B12.

Maddox, W. T., Filoteo, J. V., Hejl, K. D., and Ing, A. D. 2004. Category number impacts rule‐based, but not information‐integration category learning: Further evidence for dissociable category learning systems. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 30:227–235.

Mallon, R., Machery, E., Nichols, S., and Stich, S. P. Forthcoming. Against arguments from reference. Philosophy and Phenomenological Research.

Malt, B. C. 1989. An on‐line investigation of prototype and exemplar strategies in classification. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 15:539–555.

Malt, B. C. 1993. Concept structure and category boundaries. In The psychology of learning and motivation. Vol. 29, Categorization by humans and machines, ed. G. Nakamura, R. Taraban, and D. Medin, 363–390. Orlando, FL: Academic Press.

—— 1994. Water is not H2O. Cognitive Psychology 27:41–70.

Malt, B. C., and Johnson, E. C. 1992. Do artifact concepts have cores? Journal of Memory and Language 31:195–217.

Malt, B. C., and Sloman, S. A. 2007. Category essence or essentially pragmatic? Creator's intention in naming and what's really what. Cognition 105:615–648.

Malt, B. C., Sloman, S. A., Gennari, S., Shi, M., and Wang, Y. 1999. Knowing versus naming: Similarity and the linguistic categorization of artifacts. Journal of Memory and Language 40:230–262.

Mandler, J. M. 1992. How to build a baby: II. Conceptual primitives. Psychological Review 99:587–604.

Marcus, G. F. 2000. The algebraic mind: Integrating connectionism and cognitive science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

—— 2005. Opposites detract: Why rules and similarity should not be viewed as opposite ends of a continuum. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 28:28–29.

Margolis, E. 1994. A reassessment of the shift from the classical theory of concepts to prototype theory. Cognition 51:73–89.

—— 1995. The significance of the theory analogy in the psychological study of concepts. Mind and Language 10:45–71.

Margolis, E., and Laurence, S., eds. 1999. Concepts: Core readings. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

(p.266) Margolis, E., and Laurence, S. 2004. Concepts. In The Blackwell guide to the philosophy of mind, ed. T. A. Warfield and S. P. Stich, 190–213. Oxford: Blackwell.

Markman, A. B. 1999. Knowledge representation. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Marr, D. 1977. Artificial intelligence: A personal view. Artificial Intelligence 9:37–48.

—— 1982. Vision: A computational investigation into the human representation and processing of visual information. San Francisco, CA: W. H. Freeman.

Martin, A., and Chao, L. L. 2001. Semantic memory and the brain: Structure and processes. Current Opinion in Neurobiology 11:194–201.

McCloskey, M. 2003. Beyond task dissociation logic: A richer conception of cognitive neuropsychology. Cortex 39:196–202.

McCloskey, M., and Glucksberg, S. 1978. Natural categories: Well‐defined or fuzzy sets? Memory & Cognition 6:462–472.

McDowell, J. 1994. Mind and world. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

McNamara, T. P., and Sternberg, R. J. 1983. Mental models of word meaning. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 22:449–474.

Medin, D. L. 1989. Concepts and conceptual structure. American Psychologist 44:1469–1481.

Medin, D. L., and Atran, S., eds. 1999. Folkbiology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Medin, D. L., and Ortony, A. 1989. Psychological essentialism. In Similarity and analogical reasoning, ed. S. Vosniadou and A. Ortony, 179–195. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Medin, D. L., and Schaffer, M. M. 1978. Context theory of classification learning. Psychological Review 85:207–238.

Medin, D. L., and Schwanenflugel, P. J. 1981. Linear separability in classification learning, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory 7:355–368.

Medin, D. L., and Shoben, E. J. 1988. Context and structure in conceptual combination. Cognitive Psychology 20:158–190.

Medin, D. L., and Smith, E. E. 1984. Concepts and concept formation. Annual Review of Psychology 35:113–118.

Medin, D. L., Lynch, E. B., and Solomon, K. O. 2000. Are there kinds of concepts? Annual Review of Psychology 51:121–147.

Medin, D. L., Coley, J. D., Storms, G., and Hayes, B. 2003. A relevance theory of induction. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 3:517–532.

Mellor, J. S. 1977. Natural kinds. British Journal for the Philosophy of Science 28:299–312.

Mervis, C. B., and Rosch, E. 1981. Categorization of natural objects. Annual Review of Psychology 32:89–115.

Michalski, R. S. 1993. Beyond prototypes and frames: The two‐tiered concept representation. In Categories and concepts: Theoretical views and inductive data analysis, ed. I. Van Mechelen, J. A. Hampton, R. S. Michalski, and P. Theuns, 141–172. New York: Academic Press.

Mill, J. S. 1843. A system of logic. London: J. W. Parker.

Miller, G. A., and Johnson‐Laird, P. N. 1976. Language and perception. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Millikan, R. G. 1998. A common structure for concepts of individuals, stuffs, and real kinds: More mama, more milk, and more mouse. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 21:55–100.

(p.267) —— 2000. On clear and confused ideas: An essay about substance concepts. New York: Cambridge University Press.

Mineka, S., Davidson, M., Cook, M., and Keir, R. 1984. Observational conditioning of snake fear in rhesus monkey. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 93:355–372.

Moore, T. 1910. The process of abstraction: An experimental study.University of California Publications in Psychology 12:73–197.

Murphy, D., and Stich, S. P. 1999. Griffiths, elimination and psychopathology. Metascience 8:13–25.

Murphy, G. L. 1988. Comprehending complex concepts. Cognitive Science 12:529–562.

—— 1990. Noun phrase interpretation and conceptual combination. Journal of Memory and Language 29:259–288.

—— 2002. The big book of concepts. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Murphy, G. L., and Allopenna, P. D. 1994. The locus of knowledge effects in concept learning. (p.268) Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 20:904–919.

Murphy, G. L., and Brownell, H. H. 1985. Category differentiation in object recognition: Typicality constraints on the basic category advantage. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 11:70–84.

Murphy, G. L., and Lassaline, M. E. 1997. Hierarchical structure in concepts and the basic level of categorization. In Knowledge, concepts, and categories, ed. K. Lamberts and D. Shanks, 93–132. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Murphy, G. L., and Medin, D. L. 1985. The role of theories in conceptual coherence. Psychological Review 92:289–316.

Nazzi, T., and Gopnik, A. 2003. Sorting and acting with objects in early childhood: An exploration of the use of causal cues. Cognitive Development 18:219–237.

Newell, A., and Simon, H. A. 1976. Computer science as empirical inquiry: Symbols and search. Communications of the ACM 19:113–126.

Nichols, S., and Stich, S. P. 2003. Mindreading. New York: Oxford University Press.

Nichols, S., Stich, S. P., Leslie, A., and Klein, D. 1996. The varieties of off‐line simulation. In Theories of theories of mind, ed. P. Carruthers and P. Smith, 39–74. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Nisbett, R. E. 2003. The geography of thought: How Asians and Westerners think differently…and why. New York: Free Press.

Nisbett, R. E., Peng, K., Choi, I., and Norenzayan, A. 2001. Culture and systems of thought: Holistic vs. analytic cognition. Psychological Review 108:291–310.

Nosofsky, R. M. 1986. Attention, similarity, and the identification‐categorization relationship. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 115:39–57.

—— 1988. Exemplar‐based accounts of relations between classification, recognition, and typicality. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory & Cognition 14:700–708.

—— 1992. Exemplar‐based approach to relating categorization, identification, and recognition. In Multidimensional models of perception and cognition, ed. F. G. Ashby, 363–393. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

—— 2000. Exemplar representation without generalization? Comment on Smith and Minda's 2000 “Thirty categorization results in search of a model.” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 26:1735–1743.

Nosofsky, R. M., and Johansen, M. K. 2000. Exemplar‐based accounts of multiple‐system phenomena in perceptual categorization. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 7:375–402.

Nosofsky, R. M., and Zaki, S. R. 1998. Dissociations between categorization and recognition in amnesic and normal individuals: An exemplar‐based interpretation. Psychological Science 9:247–255.

Nosofsky, R. M., Palmeri, T. J., and McKinley, S. C. 1994. Rule‐plus‐exception model of classification learning. Psychological Review 101:53–79.

Osherson, D. N. 1978. Three conditions on conceptual naturalness. Cognition 4:263–89.

Osherson, D. N., and Smith, E. E. 1981. On the adequacy of prototype theory as a theory of concepts. Cognition 9:35–58.

—— 1997. On typicality and vagueness. Cognition 64:189–206.

Osherson, D. N., Smith, E. E., Wilkie, O., López, A., and Shafir, E. 1990. Category‐based induction. Psychological Review 97:185–200.

Osherson, D. N., Stern, J., Wilkie, O., Stob, M., and Smith, E. E. 1991. Default probability. Cognitive Science 15: 251–269.

Paivio, A. 1986. Mental representations: A dual coding approach. New York: Oxford University Press.

Palmeri, T. J. 1997. Exemplar similarity and the development of automaticity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 23:324–354.

Palmeri, T. J., and Flanery, M. A. 1999. Learning about categories in the absence of training: Profound amnesia and the relationship between perceptual categorization and recognition memory. Psychological Science 10:526–530.

Palmeri, T. J., and Gauthier, I. 2004. Visual object understanding. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 5:291–304.

Peacocke, C. 1992. A study of concepts. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

—— 1996. Can possession conditions individuate concepts? Philosophy and Phenomenological Research 56:433–460.

—— 1998. Implicit conceptions, understanding and rationality. In Philosophical issues. Vol. 9, Concepts, ed. E. Villanueva, 43–88. Atascadero, CA: Ridgeview.

—— 2004. Interrelations: Concepts, knowledge, reference and structure. Mind & Language 19:85–98.

Peterson, D. M., and Bowler, D. M. 2000. Counterfactual reasoning and false belief understanding in children with autism. Autism 4:391–405.

Peterson, D. M., and Riggs, K. J. 1999. Adaptive modeling and mindreading. Mind & Language 14:80–112.

Piaget, J. 1954. The construction of reality in the child. New York: Basic Books.

Piccinini, G., and Scott, S. 2006. Splitting concepts. Philosophy of Science 73:390–409.

Pietroski, P. 2003. The character of natural language semantics. In Epistemology of language, ed. A. Barber, 217–256. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Pinker, S. 1997. How the mind works. New York: W. W. Norton & Company.

Pinker, S., and Prince, A. 1999. The nature of human concepts: Evidence from an unusual source. In Language, logic, and concepts, ed. R. Jackendoff, P. Bloom, and K. Wynn, 221–261. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

(p.269) Plaut, D. C. 1995. Double dissociation without modularity: Evidence from connectionist neuropsychology. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology 17:291–321.

Poirier, P., and Hardy‐Vallée, B. 2005. The spatial‐motor view. In The compositionality of meaning and content: Applications to linguistics, psychology and neuroscience, ed. E. Machery, M. Werning, and G. Schurz, 229–250. Frankfurt: Ontos.

Posner, M. I., and Keele, S. W. 1968. On the genesis of abstract ideas. Journal of Experimental Psychology 77:353–363.

—— 1970. Retention of abstract ideas. Journal of Experimental Psychology 83:304–308.

Pothos, E. M. 2005. The rules versus similarity distinction. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 28:1–14.

Potter, M. C., Kroll, J. F., Yachzel, B., Carpenter, E., and Sherman, J. 1986. Pictures in sentences: Understanding without words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 115:281–294.

Prinz, J. J. 2002. Furnishing the mind: Concepts and their perceptual basis. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

—— 2004. Gut reactions. New York: Oxford University Press.

Proffitt, J. B., Coley, J. D., and Medin, D. L. 2000. Expertise and category‐based induction. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 26:811–828.

Putnam, H. 1975. The meaning of “meaning.” In Mind, language, and reality, by H. Putnam, 215–271. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Pylyshyn, Z. W. 1984. Computation and cognition: Toward a foundation for cognitive science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

—— 1999. Is vision continuous with cognition? The case for cognitive impenetrability of visual perception. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22:341–365.

—— 2003. Seeing and visualizing: It's not what you think. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Quine, W. V. O. 1951. Two dogmas of empiricism. Philosophical Review 60:20–43.

—— 1960. Word and object. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

—— 1969. Natural kinds. In Ontological relativity and other essays, by W. V. O. Quine, 114–138. New York: Columbia University Press.

Ramsey, W., Stich, S. P., and Garon, J. 1990. Connectionism, eliminativism and the future of folk psychology. Philosophical perspectives. Vol. 4, Action theory and philosophy of mind, 499–533.

Read, S. J. 1987. Constructing causal scenarios: A knowledge structure approach to causal reasoning. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 52:288–302.

Read, S. J., Jones, D. K., and Miller, L. C. 1990. Traits as goal‐based categories: The importance of goals in the coherence of dispositional categories. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 58:1048–1061.

Reber, P. J., Stark, C. E. L., and Squire, L. R. 1998. Cortical areas supporting category learning identified using functional MRI. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 95:747–750.

Reed, J. M., Squire, L. R., Patalano, A. L., Smith, E. E., and Jonides, J. 1999. Learning about categories that are defined by object‐like stimuli despite impaired declarative memory. Behavioral Neuroscience 113:411–419.

Rehder, B. 2003a. Categorization as causal reasoning. Cognitive Science, 27: 709–748.

(p.270) Rehder, B. 2003b. A causal‐model theory of conceptual representation and categorization. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 29:1141–1159.

—— 2006. When causality and similarity compete in category‐based property induction. Memory & Cognition 34:3–16.

—— 2007. Property generalization as causal reasoning. In Inductive reasoning: Experimental, developmental, and computational approaches, ed. A. Feeney and E. Heit, 81–113. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Rehder, B., and Hastie, R. 2004. Category coherence and category‐based property induction. Cognition 91:113–153.

Rehder, B., and Kim, S. 2006. How causal knowledge affects classification: A generative theory of categorization. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 32:659–683.

Rey, G. 1983. Concepts and stereotypes. Cognition 15:237–262.

—— 1985. Concepts and conceptions: A reply to Smith, Medin and Rips. Cognition 19:297–303.

Ribot, T. 1891. Enquête sur les idées générales. Revue Philosophique 32:376–388.

—— 1899. The evolution of general ideas. Trans. F. A. Welby. Chicago: Open Court Publishing Company. Translation of L'évolution des idées générales. Paris: Alcan, 1897.

Rieskamp, J., and Otto, P. E. 2006. SSL: A theory of how people learn to select strategies. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 135:207–236.

Rips, L. J. 1975. Inductive judgments about natural categories. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 14:665–681.

—— 1989. Similarity, typicality, and categorization. In Similarity and analogical reasoning, ed. S. Vosniadou and A. Ortony, 21–59. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

—— 1995. The current status of the research on concept combination. Mind & Language 10:72–104.

—— 2001. Necessity and natural categories. Psychological Bulletin 127:827–852.

Rips, L. J., Blok, S. V., and Newman, G. 2006. Tracing the identity of objects. Psychological Review 113:1–30.

Rips, L. J., Shoben, E. J., and Smith, E. E. 1973. Semantic distance and the verification of semantic relations. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 12:1–20.

Roberts, W. A. 1998. Principles of animal cognition. Boston, MA: McGraw Hill.

Roese, N. J. 1997. Counterfactual thinking. Psychological Bulletin 121:133–148.

Rogers, T. T., and McClelland, J. L. 2004. Semantic cognition: A parallel distributed processing approach. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Rosch, E. 1973. On the internal structure of perceptual and semantic categories. In Cognitive development and acquisition of language, ed. T. Moore, 111–144. New York: Academic Press.

—— 1975. Cognitive representations of semantic categories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 104:192–233.

—— 1978. Principles of categorization. In Cognition and categorization, ed. E. Rosch and B. B. Lloyd, 27–48. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Rosch, E., and Mervis, C. B. 1975. Family resemblance: Studies in the internal structure of categories. Cognitive Psychology 7:573–605.

Rosch, E., Mervis, C. B., Gray, W. D., Johnson, D., and Boyes‐Braem, P. 1976. Basic objects in natural categories. Cognitive Psychology 7:573–605.

(p.271) Ross, B. H., and Makin, V. S. 1999. Prototype versus exemplar models in cognition. In The nature of cognition, ed. R. J. Sternberg, 205–244. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Ross, B. H., Perkins, S. J., and Tenpenny, P. L. 1990. Reminding‐based category learning. Cognitive Psychology 22:460–492.

Russell, B. 1948. Human knowledge: Its scope and its limits. London: Routledge.

Ryle, G. 1951. The concept of mind. London: Hutchinson.

Salmon, W. 1971. Statistical explanation. In Statistical explanation and statistical relevance, ed. W. Salmon, 29–87. Pittsburgh: University of Pittsburgh Press.

—— 1989. Four decades of scientific explanation. In Minnesota studies in the philosophy of science, ed. P. Kitcher and W. Salmon, 13:3–219. Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota Press.

Schank, R. C., and Abelson, R. P. 1977. Scripts, plans, goals and understanding. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Schyns, P. G. 1999. The case for cognitive penetrability. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22:394.

Scott, F. J., Baron‐Cohen, S., and Leslie, A. 1999. ‘If pigs could fly’: A test of counterfactual reasoning and pretence in children with autism. British Journal of Developmental Psychology 17:349–362.

Searle, J. R. 1958. Proper names. Mind 67:166–173.

Seger, C. A., Poldrack, R. A., Prabhakaran, V., Zhao, M., Glover, G. H., Gabrieli, J. D. 2000. Hemispheric asymmetries and individual differences in visual concept learning as measured by functional MRI. Neuropsychologia 38:1316–1324.

Shallice, T. 1988. From neuropsychology to mental structure. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Shanks, D. R. 1997. Distributed representations and implicit knowledge: A brief introduction. In Knowledge, concepts, and categories, ed. K. Lamberts and D. Shanks, 197–214. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Shaver, P. R., Shelley, W., and Schwartz, J. C. 1992. Cross‐cultural similarities and differences in emotion and its representation: A prototype approach. In Review of personality and social psychology. Vol. 13, Emotion, ed. M. S. Clark, 175–212. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

Shepard, R. N. 1964. Attention and the metric structure of the stimulus space. Journal of Mathematical Psychology 1:54–87.

Shepard, R. N., and Metzler, J. 1971. Mental rotation of three dimensional objects. Science 171:701–703.

Simon, H. A. 1995. Machine as mind. In Android epistemology, ed. K. M. Ford, C. Glymour, and P. J. Hayes, 23–40. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Sloman, S. A. 1993. Feature‐based induction. Cognitive Psychology 25:231–280.

—— 1994. When explanations compete: The role of explanatory coherence on judgments of likelihood. Cognition 52:1–21.

—— 1996. The empirical case for two systems of reasoning. Psychological Bulletin 119:3–22.

—— 1998. Categorical inference is not a tree: The myth of inheritance hierarchies. Cognitive Psychology 35:1–33.

—— 2005. Causal models: How people think about the world and its alternatives. New York: Oxford University Press.

Sloman, S. A., and Lagnado, D. 2005. The problem of induction. In The Cambridge handbook of thinking & reasoning, ed. K. Holyoak and R. Morrison, 95–116. New York: Cambridge University Press.

(p.272) Sloman, S. A., and Malt, B. C. 2003. Artifacts are not ascribed essences, nor are they treated as belonging to kinds. Language and Cognitive Processes 18:563–582.

Smith, E. E. 1989. Concepts and induction. In Foundations of cognitive science, ed. M. I. Posner, 501–526. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Smith, E. E., and Medin, D. L. 1981. Categories and concepts. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Smith, E. E., and Osherson, D. N. 1989. Similarity and decision making. In Similarity and analogical reasoning, ed. S. Vosniadou and A. Ortony, 60–75. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Smith, E. E., and Sloman, S. A. 1994. Similarity‐ versus rule‐based categorization. Memory & Cognition 22:377–386.

Smith, E. E., Patalano, A. L., and Jonides, J. 1998. Alternative strategies of categorization. Cognition 65:167–196.

Smith, E. E., Shoben, E. J., and Rips, L. J. 1974. Structure and process in semantic memory: A featural model for semantic decisions. Psychological Review 81:214–241.

Smith, E. E., Osherson, D. N., Rips, L. J., and Keane, M. 1988. Combining concepts: A selective modification model. Cognitive Science 12:485–527.

Smith, J. D. 2002. Exemplar theory's predicted typicality gradient can be tested and disconfirmed. Psychological Science 13:437–442.

Smith, J. D., and Minda, J. P. 1998. Prototypes in the mist: The early epochs of category learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 24:1411–1436.

—— 2000. Thirty categorization results in search of a model. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 26:3–27.

—— 2001. Journey to the center of the category: The dissociation in amnesia between categorization and recognition.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 27:984–1002.

—— 2002. Distinguishing prototype‐based and exemplar‐based processes in category learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 28:800–811.

Smith, J. D., Murray, M. J., and Minda, J. P. 1997. Straight talk about linear separability. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 23:659–680.

Smith, L. B., and Samuelson, L. K. 1997. Perceiving and remembering: Category stability, variability and development. In Knowledge, concepts, and categories, ed. K. Lamberts and D. Shanks, 161–196. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Smoke, K. L. 1932. An objective study of concept formation. Psychological Monographs, 42:1–46.

Smolensky, P. 1991. Connectionism, constituency, and the language of thought. In Meaning in mind: Fodor and his critics, ed. B. Loewer and G. Rey, 201–227. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.

Sobel, D., Tenenbaum, J., and Gopnik, A. 2004. Children's causal inferences from indirect evidence: Backwards blocking and Bayesian reasoning in preschoolers. Cognitive Science 28:303–333.

Solomon, K. O., and Barsalou, L. W. 2001. Representing properties locally. Cognitive Psychology 43:129–169.

—— 2004. Perceptual simulation in property verification. Memory & Cognition 32:244–259.

(p.273) Solomon, K. O., Medin, D. L., and Lynch, E. L. 1999. Concepts do more than categorize. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 3:99–105.

Sousa, P., Atran, S., and Medin, D. L. 2002. Essentialism and folkbiology: Evidence from Brazil. Journal of Cognition and Culture 2:195–223.

Spalding, T. L., and Ross, B. H. 1994. Comparison‐based learning: Effects of comparing instances during category learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 20:1251–1263.

Spelke, E. S. 1994. Initial knowledge: Six suggestions. Cognition 50:431–445.

Spelke, E. S., Breinlinger, K., Macomber, J., and Jacobsen, K. 1992. Origins of knowledge. Psychological Review, 99:605–632.

Sperber, D., and Wilson, D., 1998. The mapping between the mental and the public lexicon. In Language and thought: Interdisciplinary themes, ed. P. Carruthers and J. Boucher, 184–200. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Spirtes, P., Glymour, C., and Scheines, R. 2000. Causation, prediction, and search. 2d ed. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Squire, L. R., and Knowlton, B. J. 1995. Learning about categories in the absence of memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 92:12470–12474.

Stanovich, K. E. 1999. Who is rational? Studies of individual differences in reasoning. Mahweh, NJ: Erlbaum.

Stanovich, K. E., and West, R. F. 2000. Individual differences in reasoning: Implications for the rationality debate. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 23:645–726.

—— 2003. Evolutionary versus instrumental goals: How evolutionary psychology misconceives human rationality. In Evolution and the psychology of thinking: The debate, ed. D. E. Over, 171–230. Hove: Psychology Press.

Stein, L. A. 1995. Imagination and situated cognition. In Android epistemology, ed. K. M. Ford, C. Glymour, and P. Hayes, 167–182. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Stich, S. P. 1978. Beliefs and sub‐doxastic states. Philosophy of Science 45:499–518.

—— 1983. From folk psychology to cognitive science: The case against belief. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

—— 1996. Deconstructing the mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Strevens, M. 2000. The essentialist aspect of naive theories. Cognition 74:149–175.

Taine, H. 1870. De l'intelligence. Paris: Hachette.

Tanaka, J. W. 2004. Object categorization, expertise, and neural plasticity. In The cognitive neurosciences, 3d ed., ed. M. S. Gazzaniga, 877–888. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Tenenbaum, J. B., Griffiths, T. L., and Kemp, C. 2006. Theory‐based Bayesian models of inductive learning and reasoning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 10:309–318.

Tenenbaum, J. B., Griffiths, T. L., and Niyogi, S. 2007. Intuitive theories as grammars for causal inference. In Causal learning: Psychology, philosophy, and computation, ed. A. Gopnik and L. Schulz, 301–322. New York: Oxford University Press.

Teuber, H.‐L. 1955. Physiological psychology. Annual Review of Psychology 6:267–296.

Thelen, E., and Smith, L. 1994. A dynamic systems approach to the development of cognition and action. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

(p.274) Thompson‐Schill, S. L. 2003. Neuroimaging studies of semantic memory: Inferring “how” from “where”. Neuropsychologia 41:280–292.

Thorpe, S. J., Delorme, A., and VanRullen, R. 2001. Spike based strategies for rapid processing. Neural Networks 14:715–726.

Tinbergen, N. 1963. On aims and methods in ethology. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie 20:410–433.

Todd, P. M., and Dieckmann, A. 2005. Heuristics for ordering cue search in decision making. In Advances in neural information processing systems, Vol. 17, ed. L. K. Saul, Y. Weiss, and L. Bottou, 1393–1400. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Todd, P. M., and Gigerenzer, G. 2000. Simple heuristics that make us smart. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 23:727–741.

Tversky, B., and Hemenway, K. 1984. Objects, parts, and categories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 113:169–193.

Ullman, S. 1996. High-level vision. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Van Gelder, T. J. 1990. Compositionality: A connectionist variation on a classical theme. Cognitive Science 14:355–384.

—— 1995. What might cognition be, if not computation? Journal of Philosophy 91:345–381.

Van Orden, G. C., Pennington, B. F., and Stone, G. O. 2001. What do double dissociations prove? Cognitive Science 25:111–172.

Vygotsky, L. S. 1986. Thought and language. Revised edn. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Wattenmaker, W. D., Dewey, G. I., Murphy, T. D., and Medin, D. L. 1986. Linear separability and concept learning: Context, relational properties, and concept naturalness. Cognitive Psychology 18:158–194.

Weiskopf, D. A. Forthcoming. The plurality of concepts. Synthese.

Weiskopf, D. A., and Bechtel, W. 2004. Remarks on Fodor on having concepts. Mind & Language 19:48–56.

Williamson, T. 2007. The philosophy of philosophy. Malden: Blackwell.

Wilson, T. D. 2002. Strangers to ourselves: Discovering the adaptive unconscious. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Wisniewski, E. J. 1995. Prior knowledge and functionally relevant features in concept learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 21:449–468.

Wisniewski, E. J., and Medin, D. L. 1994. On the interaction of theory and data in concept learning. Cognitive Science 18:221–281.

Wisniewski, E. J., and Wu, J. Forthcoming. Emergency!!!! Challenges to a compositional understanding of noun‐noun combinations. In The Oxford handbook of compositionality, ed. M. Werning, W. Hinzen, and E. Machery. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Wittgenstein, L. 1953. Philosophical investigations. Ed. G. E. M. Anscombe. Oxford: Blackwell.

Wood, J. N., and Grafman, J. 2003. Human prefrontal cortex function: Processing and representational perspectives. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 4:139–147.

Woodward, J. 2003a. Making things happen: A theory of causal explanation. New York: Oxford University Press.

—— 2003b. Scientific explanation. In The Stanford encyclopedia of philosophy, ed. E. N. Zalta, Winter 2003 Edition. Online. Available: http://plato.stanford.edu/entries/scientific‐explanation/. Accessed 07/24/2007.

(p.275) Wu, L. L. 1995. Perceptual representation in conceptual combination. Ph.D. diss., University of Chicago.

Young, M. P., Hilgetag, C.‐C., and Scannell, J. W. 2000. On imputing function to structure from the behavioural effects of brain lesions. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B355:147–161.

Zaki, S. R., and Nosofsky, R. M. 2001. A single‐system interpretation of dissociations between recognition and categorization in a task involving object‐like stimuli. Cognitive, Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience 1:344–359.

Zaki, S. R., Nosofsky, R. M., Jessup, N. M., and Unversagt, F. W. 2003. Categorization and recognition performance of a memory‐impaired group: Evidence for single‐system models. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society 9:394–406.

Ziff, E. 1972. Understanding understanding. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press.

Zwaan, R. A., Stanfield, R. A., and Yaxley, R. H. 2002. Language comprehenders mentally represent the shapes of objects. Psychological Science 13:168–171. (p.276)