Jump to ContentJump to Main Navigation
The Essential ChildOrigins of Essentialism in Everyday Thought$

Susan A. Gelman

Print publication date: 2003

Print ISBN-13: 9780195154061

Published to Oxford Scholarship Online: September 2007

DOI: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195154061.001.0001

Show Summary Details
Page of

PRINTED FROM OXFORD SCHOLARSHIP ONLINE (www.oxfordscholarship.com). (c) Copyright Oxford University Press, 2017. All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a monograph in OSO for personal use (for details see http://www.oxfordscholarship.com/page/privacy-policy). Subscriber: null; date: 14 December 2017

(p.335) References

(p.335) References

Source:
The Essential Child
Publisher:
Oxford University Press

Bibliography references:

Aarsleff, H. (1983). Language and Victorian ideology. American Scholar, 52, 365–372.

Abelson, R. P., and Kanouse, D. E. (1966). The subjective acceptance of verbal generalizations. In S. Feldman (Ed.), Cognitive consistency: Motivational antecedents and behavioral consequents (pp. 171–197). New York: Academic Press.

Aboud, F. E. (1988). Children and prejudice. Cambridge, MA: Basil Blackwell.

Adams, A. K., and Bullock, D. (1986). Apprenticeship in word use: Social convergence processes in learning categorically related nouns. In S. A. Kuczaj and M. D. Barrett (Eds.), The development of word meaning (pp. 155–197). New York: Springer-Verlag.

Ahn, W. (1998). The role of causal status in determining feature centrality. Cognition, 69, 135–178.

Ahn, W. (1999). Effect of causal structure on category construction. Memory and Cognition, 27, 1008–1023.

Ahn, W., Gelman, S. A., Amsterlaw, J. A., Hohenstein, J., and Kalish, C. W. (2000). Causal status effect in children's categorization. Cognition, 76, B35–B43.

Ahn, W., and Kalish, C. W. (2000). The role of mechanism beliefs in causal reasoning. In F. C. Keil and R. A. Wilson (Eds.), Explanation and cognition (pp. 199–225). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Ahn, W., Kalish, C., Gelman, S. A., Medin, D. L., Luhmann, C., Atran, S., Coley, D. J., and Shafto, P. (2001). Why essences are essential in the psychology of concepts. Cognition, 82, 59–69.

Ahn, W., Kalish, C., Medin, D. L., and Gelman, S. A. (1995). The role of covariation versus mechanism information in causal attribution. Cognition, 54, 299–352.

Ahn, W., Kim, N. S., Lassaline, M. E., and Dennis, M. J. (2000). Causal status as a determinant of feature centrality. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 361–416.

Ahn, W., and Lassaline, M. E. (1995). Causal structure in categorization. Proceedings of the Seventeenth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, Pittsburgh, PA, pp. 521–526.

Allen, S. W., and Brooks, L. R. (1991). Specializing the operation of an explicit rule. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 120, 3–19.

(p.336) Allport, G. (1954). The nature of prejudice. Addison-Wesley.

Aloise, P. A. (1993). Trait confirmation and disconfirmation: The development of attribution biases. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 55, 177–193.

Anderson, E., and Jayaratne, T. (1998). Genetic explanations of group differences: Old or new racism, sexism, and classism? Unpublished ms., University of Michigan.

Anderson, J. R. (1990). The adaptive character of thought. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Antill, J. K. (1987). Parents' beliefs and values about sex roles, sex differences, and sexuality: Their sources and implications. In P. Shaver and C. Hendrick (Eds.), Review of personality and social psychology (Vol. 7), Sex and gender (pp. 294–328). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

Aristotle. (1924). Metaphysics. Oxford: Clarendon Press.

Armstrong, S. L., Gleitman, L. R., and Gleitman, H. G. (1983). On what some concepts might not be. Cognition, 13, 263–308.

Aslin, R. N., Saffran, J. R., and Newport, E. L. (1998). Computation of conditional probability statistics by 8-month-old infants. Psychological Science, 9, 321–324.

Astuti, R. (1995). “The Vezo are not a kind of people”: Identity, difference, and “ethnicity” among a fishing people of western Madagascar. American Ethnologist, 22, 464–482.

Astuti, R. (2000). Are we all natural dualists? A cognitive developmental approach. The Malinowski Memorial Lecture.

Atran, S. (1990). Cognitive foundations of natural history. New York: Cambridge University Press.

Atran, S. (1995). Causal constraints on categories and categorical constraints on biological reasoning across cultures. In D. Sperber, D. Premack, and A. Premack (Eds.), Causal cognition: A multidisciplinary debate (pp. 205–233). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Atran, S. (1996). Modes of thinking about living kinds: Science, symbolism, and common sense. In D. Olson and N. Torrance (Eds.), Modes of thought: Explorations in culture and cognition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Atran, S. (1998). Folk biology and the anthropology of science: Cognitive universals and cultural particulars. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 21, 547–609.

Atran, S. (1999). Itzaj Maya folk-biological taxonomy. In D. Medin and S. Atran (Eds.), Folk biology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Atran, S., Estin, P., Coley, J. D., and Medin, D. L. (1997). Generic species and basic levels: Essence and appearance in folk biology. Journal of Ethnobiology, 17, 22–45.

Atran, S., Medin, D., Lynch, E., Vapnarsky, V., Ek', E. U., and Sousa, P. (2001). Folkbiology doesn't come from folkpsychology: Evidence from Yukatek Maya in cross-cultural perspective. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 1, 3–42.

Au, T. K. (1988). Language and cognition. In L. Lloyd and R. Schiefelbusch (Eds.). Language Perspectives II (pp. 125–146). Austin: Pro-Ed.

Au, T. K., Dapretto, M., and Song, Y-K. (1994). Input versus constraints: Early word acquisition in Korean and English. Journal of Memory and Language, 33, 567–582.

Au, T. K., and Romo, L. F. (1999). Mechanical causality in children's “folkbiology.” In D. L. Medin and S. Atran (Eds.), Folkbiology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Au, T. K., Sidle, A. L., and Rollins, K. B. (1993). Developing an intuitive understanding of conservation and contamination: Invisible particles as a plausible mechanism. Developmental Psychology, 29, 286–299.

Backscheider, A. G., and Gelman, S. A. (1995). Children's understanding of homonyms. Journal of Child Language, 22, 107–127.

(p.337) Backscheider, A. G., Gelman, S. A., Martinez, I., and Kowieski, J. (2000). Children's use of different information types when learning homophones and nonce words. Cognitive Development, 14, 515–530.

Backscheider, A. G., Shatz, M., and Gelman, S. A. (1993). Preschoolers' ability to distinguish living kinds as a function of self-healing. Child Development, 64, 1242–1257.

Baillargeon, R. (1993). The object concept revisited: New direction in the investigation of infants' physical knowledge. In C. Granrud (Ed.), Visual perception and cognition in infancy (265–315). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Balaban, M. T., and Waxman, S. R. (1997). Do words facilitate object categorization in 9-month-old infants? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 64, 3–26.

Baldwin, D. A. (1992). Clarifying the role of shape in children's taxonomic assumption. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 54, 392–416.

Baldwin, D. A., and Baird, J. A. (2001). Discerning intentions in dynamic human action. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5, 171–178.

Baldwin, D. A., Markman, E. M., and Melartin, R. L. (1993). Infants' ability to draw inferences about nonobvious object properties: Evidence from exploratory play. Cognitive Development, 64, 711–728.

Banton, M. (1987). Racial theories. New York: Cambridge University Press.

Bargh, J. A. (1990). Goal ≠ intent: Goal-directed thought and behavior are often unintentional. Psychological Inquiry, 1, 248–277.

Baron-Cohen, S., Tager-Flusberg, H., and Cohen, D. J. (Eds.). (1993). Understanding other minds: Perspectives from autism. New York: Oxford University Press.

Barrett, S. E., Abdi, H., Murphy, G. L., and Gallagher, J. M. (1993). Theory–based correlations and their role in children's concepts. Child Development, 64, 1595–1616.

Barsalou, L. W. (1985). Ideals, central tendency, and frequency of instantiation as determinants of graded structure in categories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 11, 629–654.

Barsalou, L. W. (1991). Deriving categories to achieve goals. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (pp. 1–64). New York: Academic Press.

Barsalou, L. W. (1992). Cognitive psychology: An overview for cognitive scientists. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Barsalou, L. W. (1993a). Flexibility, structure, and linguistic vagary in concepts: Manifestations of a compositional system of perceptual symbols. In A. C. Collins, S. E. Gather-cole, and P. E. M. Morris (Eds.), Theories of memory. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Barsalou, L. W. (1993b). Challenging assumptions about concepts. Cognitive Development, 8, 169–180.

Bartsch, K., and Wellman, H. M. (1995). Children talk about the mind. New York: Oxford University Press.

Bedard, J., and Chi, M. T. (1992). Expertise. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 1, 135–139.

Behrend, D. A. (1988). Overextensions in early language comprehension: Evidence from a signal detection approach. Journal of Child Language, 15, 63–75.

Behrend, D. A., Scofield, J., and Kleinknecht, E. E. (2001). Beyond fast mapping: Young children's extensions of novel words and novel facts. Developmental Psychology, 37, 698–705.

Bem, S. (1981). Gender schema theory: A cognitive account of sex-typing. Psychological Review, 88, 354–364.

(p.338) Bem, S. (1989). Genital knowledge and gender constancy in preschool children. Child Development, 60, 649–620.

Benjamin, W. (1955). The work of art in the age of mechanical reproduction. In H. Arendt (Ed.), Illuminations (pp. 217–251). New York: Schocken Books.

Benveniste, E. (1971). Problems in general linguistics. Coral Gables, FL: University of Miami Press.

Berlin, B. (1978). Ethnobiological classification. In E. Rosch and B. Lloyd (Eds.), Cognition and categorization. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Berlin, B. (1992). Ethnobiological classification: Principles of categorization of plants and animals in traditional societies. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

Berlin, B., Breedlove, D., and Raven, P. (1973). General principles of classification and nomenclature in folk biology. American Anthropologist, 74, 214–242.

Berndt, T., and Heller, K. (1986). Gender stereotypes and social inferences: A developmental study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 889–898.

Bertenthal, B. I. (1996). Origins and early development of perception, action, and representation. Annual Review of Psychology, 47, 431–459.

Bhat, D. N. S. (1979). The referents of noun phrases. Pune, India: Deccan College Postgraduate and Research Institute.

Bierwisch, M., and Lang, E. (Eds.) (1987). Dimensional adjectives: Grammatical structure and conceptual interpretation. Berlin: Springer-Verlag.

Bigler, R. S. (1995). The role of classification skill in moderating environmental influences on children's gender stereotyping: A study of the functional use of gender in the classroom. Child Development, 66, 1072–1087.

Bigler, R. S., Brown, C. S., and Markell, M. (2001). When groups are not created equal: Effects of group status on the formation of intergroup attitudes in children. Child Development, 72, 1151–1162.

Blewitt, P. (1983). Dog versus collie: Vocabulary in speech to young children. Developmental Psychology, 19, 602–609.

Bloch, M. (1993). Zafimaniry birth and kinship theory. Social Anthropology, 1, 119–132.

Bloch, M., Solomon, G.E.A., and Carey, S. (2001). Zafimaniry: An understanding of what is passed on from parents to children: A cross-cultural investigation. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 1, 43–68.

Bloom, A. H. (1981). The linguistic shaping of thought. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Bloom, L. (1970). Language development; form and function in emerging grammars. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Bloom, L. (1973). One word at a time. The Hague: Mouton.

Bloom, P. (1990). Syntactic distinctions in child language. Journal of Child Language, 17, 343–355.

Bloom, P. (1996). Intention, history, and artifact concepts. Cognition, 60, 1–29.

Bloom, P. (2000). How children learn the meanings of words. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Bloom, P., Abell, F., Happé, F., and Frith, U. (under review). Picture naming in children with autism.

Bloom, P., and Markson, L. (1998). Intention and analogy in children's naming of pictorial representations. Psychological Science, 9, 200–204.

Bloom, P., and Markson, L. (2001). Are there principles that apply only to the acquisition of words? A reply to Waxman and Booth. Cognition, 78, 89–90.

(p.339) Bloom, P., Markson, L., and Diesendruck, G. (1998). Origins of the shape bias. Unpublished ms., University of Arizona.

Booth, A. E., and Waxman, S. R. (2002). Word learning is “smart”: Evidence that conceptual information affects preschoolers' extension of novel words. Cognition, 84, B11–B22.

Bowerman, M. (1996). Learning how to structure space for language: A crosslinguistic perspective. In P. Bloom, M. Peterson, L. Nadel, and M. Garrett (Eds.), Language and space (pp. 385–436). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Bowerman, M. (2000). Where do children's word meanings come from? Rethinking the role of cognition in early semantic development. In L. P. Nucci, G. B. Saxe, and E. Turiel (Eds.), Culture, thought, and development (pp. 199–230). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Boyd, R. (1999). Homeostasis, species, and higher taxa. In R. A. Wilson (Ed.), Species: New interdisciplinary essays (pp. 141–185). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Boyer, P. (1990). Tradition as truth and communication: A cognitive description of traditional discourse. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Boyer, P. (1994a). The naturalness of religious ideas: A cognitive theory of religion. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.

Boyer, P. (1994b). Cognitive constraints on cultural representations: Natural ontologies and religious ideas. In L. A. Hirschfeld and S. A. Gelman (Eds.), Mapping the mind: Domain specificity in cognition and culture (pp. 391–411). New York: Cambridge University Press.

Brace, C. L. (1964). A nonracial approach towards the understanding of human diversity. In A. Montagu (Ed.), The concept of race. New York: The Free Press.

Braisby, N., Franks, B., and Hampton, J. (1996). Essentialism, word use, and concepts. Cognition, 59, 247–274.

Bressan, P., and Dal Martello, M. F. (2002). Talis pater, talis filius: Perceived resemblance and the belief in genetic relatedness. Psychological Science, 13, 213–218.

Brooks, L. R., and Wood, T. (1997). Identification in service of use: Characteristic of everyday concept learning. Paper presented at the 38th annual meeting of the Psychonomic Society.

Brooks, P. J., and Braine, M.D.S. (1996). What do children know about the universal quantifiers all and each? Cognition 60, 235–268.

Brown, A. L. (1990). Domain-specific principles affect learning and transfer in children. Cognitive Science, 14, 107–133.

Brown, A., and Kane, M. (1988). Preschool children can learn to transfer: Learning to learn and learning from example. Cognitive Psychology, 20, 493–523.

Brown, R. (1957). Linguistic determinism and the part of speech. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 55, 1–5.

Brown, R. (1976). Reference: In memorial tribute to Eric Lenneberg. Cognition, 4, 125–153.

Brown, R. W. (1973). A first language: The early stages. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Bruner, J. S. (1973). Beyond the information given. New York: Norton.

Bruner, J. S., Goodnow, J.J., and Austin, G. A. (Eds.). (1956). A study of thinking. New York: Wiley.

Bruner, J. S., Olver, R. R., Greenfield, P. M., et al. (1966). Studies in cognitive growth. New York: John Wiley.

Bullock, M., Gelman, R., and Baillargeon, R. (1982). The development of causal reasoning. (p.340) In W. J. Friedman (Ed.), The developmental psychology of time (pp. 209–254). New York: Academic Press.

Cain, K. M., and Dweck, C. S. (1995). The relation between motivational patterns and achievement cognitions through the elementary school years. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 41, 25–52.

Cain, K. M., Heyman, G. D., and Walker, M. E. (1997). Preschoolers' ability to make dispositional predictions within and across domains. Social Development, 6, 53–75.

Callanan, M. A. (1985). How parents label objects for young children: The role of input in the acquisition of category hierarchies. Child Development, 56, 508–523.

Callanan, M. A. (1989). Development of object categories and inclusion relations: Preschoolers' hypotheses about word meanings. Developmental Psychology, 25, 207–216.

Callanan, M. A. (1990). Parents' descriptions of objects: Potential data for children's inferences about category principles. Cognitive Development, 5, 101–122.

Callanan, M. A., and Oakes, L. M. (1992). Preschoolers' questions and parents' explanations: Causal thinking in everyday activity. Cognitive Development, 7, 213–233.

Cannon, J. (1993). Stellaluna. New York: Harcourt, Inc.

Carey, S. (1985). Conceptual development in childhood. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Carey, S. (1995). On the origins of causal understanding. In D. Sperber, D. Premack, and A. J. Premack (Eds.), Causal cognition: A multi-disciplinary approach (pp. 268–308). Oxford: Clarendon Press.

Carey, S. (1996). Cognitive domains as modes of thought. In D. R. Olson and N. Torrance (Eds.), Modes of thought: Explorations in culture and cognition (pp. 187–215). New York: Cambridge University Press.

Carey, S., and Spelke, E. (1994). Domain-specific knowledge and conceptual change. In L. A. Hirschfeld and S. A. Gelman (Eds.), Mapping the mind: Domain specificity in cognition and culture (pp. 169–200). New York: Cambridge University Press

Carey, S., and Xu, F. (1999). Sortals and kinds: An appreciation of John Macnamara. In R. Jackendoff, P. Bloom, and K. Wynn (Eds.), Language, logic, and concepts: Essays in memory of John Macnamara. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Carlson, G. N. (1977). A unified analysis of the English bare plural. Linguistics and Philosophy, 1, 413–456.

Carlson, G. N., and Pelletier, F. J. (Eds.) (1995). The generic book. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Chandler, M. J., and Lalonde, C. E. (1994). Surprising, magical and miraculous turns of events: Children's reactions to violations of their early theories of mind and matter. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 12, 83–95.

Chi, M. T. H., Hutchinson, J. E., and Robin, A. F. (1989). How inferences about novel domain-related concepts can be constrained by structured knowledge. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 35, 27–62.

Chi, M. T. H., and Koeske, R. (1983). Network representation of a child's dinosaur knowledge. Developmental Psychology, 19, 29–39.

Choi, I., Nisbett, R. E., and Smith, E. E. (1997). Culture, categorization, and inductive reasoning. Cognition, 65, 15–32.

Choi, S. (2000). Caregiver input in English and Korean: Use of nouns and verbs in book-reading and toy-play contexts. Journal of Child Language, 27, 69–96.

Choi, S., and Gopnik, A. (1995). Early acquisition of verbs in Korean: A crosslinguistic study. Journal of Child Language, 22, 497–529.

(p.341) Chomsky, N. (1988). Language and problems of knowledge. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Christiansen, M. H., Allen, J., and Seidenberg, M. S. (1998). Learning to segment speech using multiple cues: A connectionist model. Language and Cognitive Processes, 13, 221–268.

Clark, E. V. (1973). What's in a word? On the child's acquisition of semantics in his first language. In T. E. Moore (Ed.), Cognitive development and the acquisition of language. New York: Academic Press.

Clark, E. V. (1987). The principle of contrast: A constraint on language acquisition. In B. MacWhinney (Ed.), Mechanisms of language acquisition (pp. 1–33). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Clark, E. V., Gelman, S. A., and Lane, N. (1985). Noun compounds and category structure in young children. Child Development, 56, 84–94.

Cohen, L. B., and Oakes, L. M. (1993). How infants perceive a simple causal event. Developmental Psychology, 29, 421–433.

Coley, J. D. (1993). Emerging differentiation of folkbiology and folkpsychology: Similarity judgments and property attributions. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor.

Coley, J. D. (2000). On the importance of comparative research: The case of folkbiology. Child Development, 71, 82–90.

Coley, J. D., and Gelman, S. A. (1989). The effects of object orientation and object type on children's interpretation of the word “big.” Child Development, 60, 372–380.

Coley, J. D., and Luhmann, C. C. (2001). Domain specific relations between typicality and absolute category membership. Unpublished ms., Northeastern University.

Coley, J. D., Medin, D. L, and Atran, S. (1997). Does rank have its privilege? Inductive inferences within folkbiological taxonomies. Cognition, 64, 73–112.

Coley, J. D., Medin, D. L., Proffitt, J. B., Lynch, E., and Atran, S. (1999). Inductive reasoning in folkbiological thought. In D. L. Medin and S. Atran (Eds.), Folkbiology (pp. 205–232). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Crocker, J. C. (1979). Selves and alters among the Eastern Bororo. In D. Maybury-Lewis (Ed.), Dialectical societies: The Ge and Bororo of Central Brazil (pp. 249–300). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Croft, W. (1990). Typology and universals. New York: Cambridge University Press.

Cunningham, C. J. (1999). Illnesses as labels: The influence of linguistic form class. Undergraduate honors thesis, University of Michigan.

Dahl, O. (1975). On generics. In E. L. Keenan (Ed.), Formal semantics of natural language (pp. 99–111). Cambridge University Press.

Daniel, E. V. (1984). Fluid signs: Being a person the Tamil way. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.

Danziger, E. (2001). Relatively speaking: Language, thought, and kinship among the Mopan Maya. New York: Oxford University Press.

Darley, J. M., and Fazio, R. H. (1980). Expectancy-confirmation processes arising in the social interaction sequence. American Psychologist, 35, 867–881.

Davidson, N. S., and Gelman, S. A. (1990). Inductions from novel categories: The role of language and conceptual structure. Cognitive Development, 5, 151–176.

Deák, G., and Bauer, P. J. (1995). The effects of task comprehension on preschoolers' and adults' categorization choices. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 60, 393–427.

Deák, G., and Bauer, P. J. (1996). The dynamics of preschoolers' categorization choices. Child Development, 67, 140–168.

(p.342) DeLoache, J. S. (1991). Symbolic functioning in very young children: Understanding of pictures and models. Child Development, 62, 736–752.

DeLoache, J. S., and DeMendoza, O. A. P. (1987). Joint picturebook interactions of mothers and 1-year-old children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 5, 111–123.

Dennett, D. C. (1995). Darwin's dangerous idea: Evolution and the meanings of life. New York: Simon and Schuster.

DeVries, R. (1969). Constancy of generic identity in the years three to six. Society for Research in Child Development Monographs, 34 (Whole No. 127).

Diesendruck, G. (2001). Essentialism in Brazilian children's extensions of animal names. Developmental Psychology, 37, 49–60.

Diesendruck, G. (in press). Categories for names or names for categories? The interplay between domain-specific conceptual structure and language. Language and Cognitive Processes.

Diesendruck, G., and Gelman, S. A. (1999). Domain differences in absolute judgments of category membership: Evidence for an essentialist account of categorization. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 6, 338–346.

Diesendruck, G., Gelman, S. A., and Lebowitz, K. (1998). Conceptual and linguistic biases in children's word learning. Developmental Psychology, 34, 823–839.

Diesendruck, G., and Markson, L. (1999, April). Function as a criterion in children's object naming. Poster presented at the Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Albuquerque, NM.

Diesendruck, G., and Shatz, M. (1997). The effect of perceptual similarity and linguistic input on children's acquisition of object labels. Journal of Child Language, 24, 695–717.

Diesendruck, G., and Shatz, M. (2001). Two-year-olds' recognition of hierarchies: Evidence from their interpretation of the semantic relation between object labels. Cognitive Development, 16, 577–594.

Disney, W. (1977). Lambert the sheepish lion. New York: Random House.

Donaldson, M., and McGarrigle, J. (1974). Some clues to the nature of semantic development. Journal of Child Language, 1, 185–194.

Douglas, M. (1966). Purity and danger: An analysis of concepts of pollution and taboo. New York: Praeger.

Dozier, M. (1991). Functional measurement assessment of young children's ability to predict future behavior. Child Development, 62, 1091–1099.

Droege, K. L., and Stipek, D. J. (1993). Children's use of dispositions to predict classmates' behavior. Developmental Psychology, 29, 646–654.

Dromi, E. (1987). Early lexical development. Cambridge University Press.

Dupré, J. (1993). The disorder of things: Metaphysical foundations of the disunity of science. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Dweck, C. S. (1999). Self-theories: Their role in motivation, personality, and development. Philadelphia: Psychology Press.

Eakin, E. (2001, February 24). Screwdriver scholars and pencil punditry. New York Times.

Eder, R. A. (1989). The emergent personologist: The structure and content of 3?-, 5?-, and 7?-year-olds' concepts of themselves and other persons. Child Development, 60, 1218–1228.

Eimas, P. D., and Quinn, P. C. (1994). Studies on the formation of perceptually based basiclevel categories in young infants. Child Development, 65, 903–917.

(p.343) Ellis, B. D. (2001). Scientific essentialism. New York: Cambridge University Press.

Ellis, L. (1998). The evolution of attitudes about social stratification: Why many people (including social scientists) are morally outraged by The Bell Curve. Personality and Individual Differences, 24, 207–216.

Emmerich, W., Goldman, K. S., and Kirsh, B. (1977). Evidence for a transitional phase in the development of gender constancy. Child Development, 48, 930–936.

Errington, S. (1989). Meaning and power in a Southeast Asian realm. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

Estes, W. K. (1994). Classification and cognition. New York: Oxford University Press.

Evans, E. M. (2000). Beyond Scopes: Why creationism is here to stay. In K. S. Rosengren, C. N. Johnson, and P. L. Harris (Eds.), Imagining the impossible (pp. 305–333). New York: Cambridge University Press.

Evans, E. M. (2001). Cognitive and contextual factors in the emergence of diverse belief systems: Creation versus evolution. Cognitive Psychology, 42, 217–266.

Fairchild, C. (1984). Domestic enemies: Servants and their masters in old regime France. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins Press.

Farrar, M. J., Raney, G. E., and Boyer, M. E. (1992). Knowledge, concepts, and inferences in childhood. Child Development, 63, 673–691.

Flavell, J. H. (1963). The developmental psychology of Jean Piaget. Princeton, NJ: Van Nostrand.

Flavell, J. H. (1977). Cognitive Development. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

Flavell, J. H. (1985). Cognitive development, 2nd ed. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

Flavell, J. H., Flavell, E. R., Green, F. L. (1983). Development of the appearance-reality distinction. Cognitive Psychology, 15, 95–120.

Flavell, J. H., Flavell, E. R., Green, F. L., and Moses, L. J. (1990). Young children's understanding of fact beliefs versus value beliefs. Child Development, 61, 915–928.

Flavell, J. H., and Miller, P. H. (1998). Social cognition. In D. Kuhn and R. Siegler (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology, 4th ed., Cognitive development (pp. 851–898). New York: Wiley.

Florian, J. E. (1994). Stripes do not a zebra make, or do they? Conceptual and perceptual information in inductive inference. Developmental Psychology, 30, 88–101.

Fodor, J. (1975). The language of thought. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Fodor, J. (1998). Concepts: Where cognitive science went wrong. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Fugelsang, J. A., and Thompson, V. A. (2000). Strategy selection in causal reasoning: When beliefs and covariation collide. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54, 15–32.

Fuss, D. (1989). Essentially speaking: Feminism, nature, and difference. New York: Routledge.

Gelman, R. (1978). Cognitive development. Annual Review of Psychology, 29, 297–332.

Gelman, R. (1987, August). Cognitive development: Principles guide learning and contribute to conceptual coherence. Paper presented at the meetings of the American Psychological Association, Division I, New York.

Gelman, R. (1990). First principles organize attention to and learning about relevant data: Number and the animate-inanimate distinction as examples. Cognitive Science, 14, 79–106.

Gelman, R., Durgin, F., and Kaufman, L. (1995). Distinguishing between animates and inanimates: Not by motion alone. In D. Sperber, D. Premack, and A. J. Premack (p.344) (Eds.), Causal cognition: A multidisciplinary debate (pp. 150–184). Oxford: Clarendon Press.

Gelman, S. A. (1988). The development of induction within natural kind and artifact categories. Cognitive Psychology, 20, 65–96.

Gelman, S. A. (1992). Children's conception of personality traits—commentary. Human Development, 35, 280–285.

Gelman, S. A. (1993). Children's conceptions of biological growth. In J. Montangero and A. Tryphon (Eds.), Conceptions of change over time (pp. 197–208). Cahiers de la Foundation Archives Jean Piaget No. 13.

Gelman, S. A. (1996). Concepts and theories. In R. Gelman and T. Au (Eds.), Handbook of perception and cognition volume 13: Perceptual and cognitive development.

Gelman, S. A. (2000). The role of essentialism in children's concepts. In H. W. Reese (Ed.), Advances in child development and behavior, Vol. 27 (pp. 55–98). San Diego: Academic Press.

Gelman, S. A. (in press). Learning words for kinds: Generic noun phrases in acquisition. To appear in D. G. Hall and S. R. Waxman (Eds.), Weaving a lexicon. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Gelman, S. A., and Bloom, P. (2000). Young children are sensitive to how an object was created when deciding what to name it. Cognition, 76, 91–103.

Gelman, S. A., and Coley, J. D. (1990). The importance of knowing a dodo is a bird: Categories and inferences in 2-year-old children. Developmental Psychology, 26, 796–804.

Gelman, S. A., and Coley, J. D. (1991). Language and categorization: The acquisition of natural kind terms. In S. A. Gelman and J. P. Byrnes (Eds.), Perspectives on language and thought: Interrelations in development (pp. 146–196). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Gelman, S. A., Coley, J. D., and Gottfried, G. M. (1994). Essentialist beliefs in children: The acquisition of concepts and theories. In L. A. Hirschfeld and S. A. Gelman (Eds.), Mapping the mind: Domain specificity in cognition and culture (pp. 341–365). Cambridge University Press.

Gelman, S. A., Coley, J. D., Rosengren, K., Hartman, E., and Pappas, T. (1998). Beyond labeling: The role of parental input in the acquisition of richly structured categories. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. Serial No. 253, Vol. 63, No. 1.

Gelman, S. A., Collman, P., and Maccoby, E. E. (1986). Inferring properties from categories versus inferring categories from properties: The case of gender. Child Development, 57, 396–404.

Gelman, S. A., Croft, W., Fu, P., Clausner, T., and Gottfried, G. (1998). Why is a pomegranate an apple? The role of shape, taxonomic relatedness, and prior lexical knowledge in children's overextensions of apple and dog. Journal of Child Language, 25, 267–291.

Gelman, S. A., and Diesendruck, G. (1999). What's in a concept? Context, variability, and psychological essentialism. In I. E. Sigel (Ed.), Development of mental representation: Theories and applications (pp. 87–111). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Gelman, S. A., and Ebeling, K. S. (1998). Shape and representational status in children's early naming. Cognition, 66, B35–B47.

Gelman, S. A., and Gottfried, G. (1996). Causal explanations of animate and inanimate motion. Child Development, 67, 1970–1987.

Gelman, S. A., and Heyman, G. D. (1999). Carrot-eaters and creature-believers: The effects of (p.345) lexicalization on children's inferences about social categories. Psychological Science, 10, 489–493.

Gelman, S. A., and Hirschfeld, L. A. (1999). How biological is essentialism? In S. Atran and D. Medin (Eds.), Folk biology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Gelman, S. A., Hollander, M., Star, J., and Heyman, G. D. (2000). The role of language in the construction of kinds. In D. Medin (Ed.), Psychology of learning and motivation, Vol. 39 (pp. 201–263). New York: Academic Press.

Gelman, S. A., and Kalish, C. W. (1993). Categories and causality. In R. Pasnak and M. L. Howe (Eds.), Emerging themes in cognitive development, Vol. II: Competencies (pp. 3–32). New York: Springer-Verlag.

Gelman, S. A., and Koenig, M. A. (in press). Theory-based categorization in early childhood. In D. H. Rakison and L. M. Oakes (Eds.), Early category and concept development: Making sense of the blooming, buzzing confusion. New York: Oxford University Press.

Gelman, S. A., and Kremer, K. E. (1991). Understanding natural cause: Children's explanations of how objects and their properties originate. Child Development, 62, 396–414.

Gelman, S. A., and Markman, E. M. (1985). Implicit contrast in adjectives versus nouns: Implications for word-learning in preschoolers. Journal of Child Language, 12, 125–143.

Gelman, S. A., and Markman, E. M. (1986). Categories and induction in young children. Cognition, 23, 183–209.

Gelman, S. A., and Markman, E. M. (1987). Young children's inductions from natural kinds: The role of categories and appearances. Child Development, 58, 1532–1541.

Gelman, S. A., and Medin, D. L. (1993). What's so essential about essentialism? A different perspective on the interaction of perception, language, and conceptual knowledge. Cognitive Development, 8, 157–167.

Gelman, S. A., Nguyen, S. P., and Taylor, M. G. (2001). Maternal talk about gender to 2-, 4-, and 6-year-old children. Unpublished data, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor.

Gelman, S. A., and O'Reilly, A. W. (1988). Children's inductive inferences within superordinate categories: The role of language and category structure. Child Development, 59, 876–887.

Gelman, S. A., and Opfer, J. (2002). Development of the animate-inanimate distinction. In U. Goswami, Handbook of Cognitive Development (pp. 151–166). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.

Gelman, S. A., and Raman, L. (in press). Preschool children use linguistic form class and pragmatic cues to interpret generics. Child Development.

Gelman, S. A., Star, J., and Flukes, J. (2002). Children's use of generics in inductive inferences. Journal of Cognition and Development, 3, 179–199.

Gelman, S. A., and Tardif, T. Z. (1998). Generic noun phrases in English and Mandarin: An examination of child-directed speech. Cognition, 66, 215–248.

Gelman, S. A., and Taylor, M. (1984). How 2-year-old children interpret proper and common names for unfamiliar objects. Child Development, 55, 1535–1540.

Gelman, S. A., and Taylor, M. G. (2000). Gender essentialism in cognitive development. In P. H. Miller and E. K. Scholnick (Eds.), Toward a feminist developmental psychology (pp. 169–190). New York: Routledge.

Gelman, S. A., and Wellman, H. M. (1991). Insides and essences: Early understandings of the nonobvious. Cognition, 38, 213–244.

Gelman, S. A., Wilcox, S. A., and Clark, E. V. (1989). Conceptual and linguistic hierarchies in young children. Cognitive Development, 4, 309–326.

(p.346) Gentner, D. (1982). Why nouns are learned before verbs: Linguistic relativity versus natural partitioning. In S. A. Kuczaj II (Ed.), Language development: Syntax and semantics. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Gentner, D., and Imai, M. (1994). A further examination of the shape bias in early word learning. Proceedings of the Child Language Research Forum. Stanford, CA.

Gentner, D., and Medina, J. (1998). Similarity and the development of rules. Cognition, 65, 263–297.

Giles, J. W., and Heyman, G. D. (2001, April). Preschoolers' social beliefs as predictors of their social competence. Paper presented at the Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Minneapolis, MN.

Gil-White, F. J. (2001). Are ethnic groups biological “species” to the human brain? Current Anthropology, 42, 515–554.

Glucksberg, S. (2001). Understanding figurative language: From metaphors to idioms. New York: Oxford University Press.

Gobbo, C., and Chi, M. (1986). How knowledge is structured and used by expert and novice children. Cognitive Development, 1, 221–237.

Goldberg, T. (1993). Racist culture: Philosophy and the politics of meaning. Blackwell.

Goldin-Meadow, S., and Mylander, C. (1990). Beyond the input given: The child's role in the acquisition of language. Language, 66, 323–355.

Golinkoff, R. M., and Hirsh-Pasek, K. (2000). Word learning: Icon, index, or symbol? In Becoming a word learner: A debate on lexical acquisition (pp. 3–18). New York: Oxford University Press.

Golinkoff, R. M., Mervis, C. B., and Hirsh-Pasek, K. (1994). Early object labels: The case for a developmental lexical principles framework. Journal of Child Language, 21, 125–155.

Golinkoff, R. M., Shuff-Bailey, M., Olguin, R., and Ruan, W. (1995). Young children extend novel words at the basic-level: Evidence for the principle of categorical scope. Developmental Psychology, 31, 494–507.

Gomez, R. L., and Gerken, L. (1999). Artificial grammar learning by 1-year-olds leads to specific and abstract knowledge. Cognition, 70, 109–135.

Goodman, N. (1972). Seven strictures on similarity. In N. Goodman (Ed.), Problems and project. Bobbs-Merrill.

Goodman, N. (1973). Fact, fiction, and forecast (3rd ed.). Indianapolis: Bobbs-Merrill.

Gopnik, A. (2000). Explanation as orgasm and the drive for causal knowledge: The function, evolution, and phenomenology of the theory formation system. In F. C. Keil and R. A. Wilson (Eds.), Explanation and cognition. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Gopnik, A., and Choi, S. (1990). Do linguistic differences lead to cognitive differences? A cross-linguistic study of semantic and cognitive development. First Language, 10, 199–215.

Gopnik, A., Choi, S., and Baumberger, T. (1996). Cross-linguistic differences in early semantic and cognitive development. Cognitive Development, 11, 197–227.

Gopnik, A., and Meltzoff, A. N. (1997). Words, thoughts, and theories. Cambridge, MA: Bradford Books/MIT Press.

Gopnik, A., and Sobel, D. M. (2000). Detecting blickets: How young children use information about novel causal powers in categorization and induction. Child Development, 71, 1205–1222.

Gopnik, A., and Wellman, H. (1994). The theory theory. In L. A. Hirschfeld and S. A. Gelman (p.347) (Eds.), Mapping the mind: Domain specificity in cognition and culture. NY: Cambridge University Press.

Goswami, U. (1996). Analogical reasoning and cognitive development. In H. W. Reese (Ed.), Advances in Child Development and Behavior, Vol. 26 (pp. 92–138). San Diego: Academic Press.

Gottfried, G. M., Gelman, S. A., and Shultz, J. (1999). Children's understanding of the brain: From early essentialism to biological theory. Cognitive Development, 14, 147–174.

Gottfried, G. M., and Tonks, S. J. M. (1996). Specifying the relation between novel and known: Input affects the acquisition of novel color terms. Child Development, 67, 850–866.

Gould, S. J. (1989). Wonderful life. New York: W. W. Norton and Co.

Graham, S. A., Kilbreath, C. S., and Welder, A. N. (2001). Words and shape similarity guide 13-month-olds' inferences about nonobvious object properties. In J. D. Moore and K. Stenning (Eds.), Proceedings of the Twenty-Third Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 352–357). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Graham, S. A., Kilbreath, C. S., and Welder, A. N. (2002). The importance of being a FLUM: 12-month-olds rely on shared labels and shape similarity for inductive inferences. Paper presented at the Thirteenth Biennial International Conference on Infant Studies, Toronto, Canada.

Green, M. (1978). Structure and sequence in children's concepts of chance and probability: A replication study of Piaget and Inhelder. Child Development, 49, 1045–1053.

Greenberg, J. H. (1966). Language universals. The Hague: Mouton.

Guillaumin, C. (1980). The idea of race and its elevation to autonomous scientific and legal status. In Sociological theories: Race and colonialism (pp. 37–68). Paris: UNESCO.

Gumperz, J. J., and Levinson, S. C. (Eds.). (1996). Rethinking linguistic relativity. New York: Cambridge University Press.

Gunnar-von Gnechten, M. R. (1978). Changing a frightening toy into a pleasant toy by allowing the infant to control its actions. Developmental Psychology, 14, 157–162.

Gutheil, D. G. (1993). Preschoolers' understanding of individual identity stability in simple objects. Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, University of Michigan.

Gutheil, G., and Gelman, S. A. (1997). Children's use of sample size and diversity information within basic-level categories. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 64, 159–174.

Gutheil, G., and Rosengren, K. S. (1996). A rose by any other name: Preschooolers' understanding of individual identity across name and appearance changes. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 14, 477–498.

Hall, D. G. (1993). Basic-level individuals. Cognition, 48, 199–221.

Hall, D. G. (1994). How mothers teach basic-level and situation-restricted count nouns. Journal of Child Language, 21, 391–414.

Hall, D. G. (1996). Preschoolers' default assumptions about word meaning: Proper names designate unique individuals. Developmental Psychology, 32, 177–186.

Hall, D. G. (1999). Semantics and the acquisition of proper names. In R. Jackendoff, P. Bloom, and K. Wynn (Eds.), Language, logic, and concepts: Essays in memory of John Macnamara. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Hall, D. G., and Moore, C. E. (1997). Red bluebirds and black greenflies: Preschoolers' understanding of the semantics of adjectives and count nouns. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 67, 236–267.

(p.348) Hall, D. G., and Waxman, S. R. (1993). Assumptions about word meaning: Individuation and basic-level kinds. Child Development, 64, 1550–1570.

Hall, D. G., Waxman, S. R., and Hurwitz, W. R. (1993). How 2- and 4-year-old children interpret adjectives and count nouns. Child Development, 64, 1661–1664.

Hallett, G. L. (1991). Essentialism: A Wittgensteinian critique. Albany, NY: SUNY Press.

Hamilton, D. L., and Sherman, S. J. (1996). Perceiving persons and groups. Psychological Review, 103, 336–355.

Hamilton, D. L., Sherman, S. J., and Lickel, B. (1998). Perceiving social groups: The importance of the entitativity continuum. In C. Sedikides, J. Schopler, and C. A. Insko (Eds.), Intergroup cognition and intergroup behavior. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Hampton, J. A. (1982). A demonstration of intransitivity in natural categories. Cognition, 12, 151–164.

Hampton, J. A. (1998). Similarity-based categorization and fuzziness of natural categories. Cognition, 65, 137–165.

Hamad, S. (1987). Categorical perception: The groundwork of cognition. New York: Cambridge University Press.

Harre, R., and Madden, E. H. (1975). Causal powers: A theory of natural necessity. Oxford: Blackwell.

Haslam, N. (1998). Natural kinds, human kinds, and essentialism. Social Research, 65, 291–314.

Haslam, N., and Ernst, D. (2002). Essentialist beliefs about mental disorders. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 21.

Haslam, N., Rothschild, L., and Ernst, D. (2000). Essentialist beliefs about social categories. British Journal of Social Psychology, 39, 113–127.

Haslam, N., Rothschild, L., and Ernst, D. (2002). Are essentialist beliefs associated with prejudice? British Journal of Social Psychology, 41, 87–100.

Hatano, G., and Inagaki, K. (1999). A developmental perspective on informal biology. In D. L. Medin and S. Atran (Eds.), Folkbiology (pp. 321–354). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Hauser, M. D. (1997). Artifactual kinds and functional design features: What a primate understands without language. Cognition, 64, 285–308.

Hayne, H., Rovee-Collier, C., and Perris, E. E. (1987). Categorization and memory retrieval by three-month-olds. Child Development, 58, 750–767.

Heath, S. B. (1983). Ways with words. Cambridge University Press.

Heit, E., and Hahn, U. (2001). Diversity-based reasoning in children. Cognitive Psychology, 43, 243–273.

Heit, E., and Rubinstein, J. (1994). Similarity and property effects in inductive reasoning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20, 411–422.

Heller, K. A., and Berndt, T. J. (1981). Developmental changes in the formation and organization of personality attributions. Child Development, 52, 683–691.

Helweg, H. (1978). Farm animals. New York: Random House.

Herey, G. (1985). Generic descriptions, default reasoning, and typicality. Theoretical Linguistics, 12, 33–72.

Herrnstein, R. J., and de Villiers, P. A. (1980). Fish as a natural category for people and pigeons. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation, 14, 59–95. New York: Academic Press.

Herrnstein, R. J., Loveland, D. H., and Cable, C. (1976). Natural concepts in pigeons. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 2, 285–302.

(p.349) Heyman, G. D., and Diesendruck, G. (in press). The Spanish ser/estar distinction in bilingual children's reasoning about human psychological characteristics. Developmental Psychology.

Heyman, G. D., Dweck, C. S., and Cain, K. M. (1992). Young children's vulnerability to self-blame and helplessness: Relationship to beliefs about goodness. Child Development, 63, 401–415.

Heyman, G. D., and Gelman, S. A. (1998). Young children use motive information to make trait inferences. Developmental Psychology, 34, 310–321.

Heyman, G., and Gelman, S. A. (1999). The use of trait labels in making psychological inferences. Child Development, 70, 604–619.

Heyman, G. D., and Gelman, S. A. (2000a). Beliefs about the origins of human psychological traits. Developmental Psychology, 36, 665–678.

Heyman, G. D. and Gelman, S. A. (2000b). Preschool children's use of novel predicates to make inductive inferences about people. Cognitive Development, 15, 263–280.

Heyman, G. D., and Gelman, S. A. (2000c). Preschool children's use of trait labels to make inductive inferences. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 77, 1–19.

Hickling, A. K., and Gelman, S. A. (1995). How does your garden grow? Evidence of an early conception of plants as biological kinds. Child Development, 66, 856–876.

Hickling, A. K., and Wellman, H. M. (2001). The emergence of children's causal explanations and theories: Evidence from everyday conversation. Developmental Psychology, 37, 668–683.

Hill, J., and Mannheim, B. (1992). Language and world view. Annual Review of Anthropology, 21, 381–406.

Hird, M. J., and Germon, J. (2001). The intersexual body and the medical regulation of gender. In K. Backett-Milburn and L. McKie (Eds.), Constructing gendered bodies. New York: Palgrave.

Hirschfeld, L. A. (1995). Do children have a theory of race? Cognition, 54, 209–252.

Hirschfeld, L. A. (1996). Race in the making: Cognition, culture, and the child's construction of human kinds. Cambridge: MIT Press.

Hirschfeld, L. A. (2002). Why don't anthropologists like children? American Anthropologist, 104.

Hirschfeld, L. A., and Gelman, S. A. (1997). What young children think about the relation between language variation and social difference. Cognitive Development, 12, 213–238.

Hoffner, C., and Cantor, J. (1985). Developmental differences in responses to a television character's appearance and behavior. Developmental Psychology, 21, 1065–1074.

Holland, J. H., Holyoak, K. J., Nisbett, R. E., and Thagard, P. R. (1986). Induction: Processes of inference, learning, and discovery. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Hollander, M. A., Gelman, S. A., and Star, J. (in press). Children's interpretation of generic noun phrases. Developmental Psychology.

Holmes, W. G., and Sherman, P. W. (1983). Kin recognition in animals. American Scientist, 71, 46–55.

Hull, C. L. (1920). Quantitative aspects of the evolution of concepts, an experimental study. Princeton, NJ: Psychological Review Company.

Hunn, E. S. (1998). Atran's biodiversity parser: Doubts about hierarchy and autonomy. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 21, 576–577.

Hutchinson, J. E., and Herman, J. P. (1991). The development of word-learning strategies in (p.350) delayed children. Paper presented at the Boston University Conference on Language Development.

Hutchinson, J., Inn, D., and Strapp, C. (1993). A longitudinal study of one year–olds' acquisition of the mutual exclusivity and lexical gap assumptions. Paper presented at the Stanford Child Language Research Forum.

Imai, M. (1995). Asymmetry in the taxonomic assumption: Word learning versus property induction. In E. V. Clark (Ed.), The proceedings of the 27th annual Child Language Research Forum. Chicago, IL: Center for the Study of Language and Information.

Imai, M., and Gentner, D. (1997). A cross–linguistic study of early word meaning: Universal ontology and linguistic influence. Cognition, 62, 169–200.

Imai, M., Gentner, D., and Uchida, N. (1994). Children's theories of word meaning: The role of shape similarity in early acquisition. Cognitive Development, 9, 45–75.

Inagaki, K., and Hatano, G. (1993). Young children's understanding of the mind–body distinction. Child Development, 64, 1534–1549.

Inagaki, K., and Hatano, G. (1996). Young children's recognition of commonalities between animals and plants. Child Development, 67, 2823–2840.

Inagaki, K., and Hatano, G. (2002). Young children's naive thinking about the biological world. New York: Psychology Press.

Inhelder, B., and Piaget, J. (1958). The growth of logical thinking from childhood to adolescence. New York: Basic Books.

Inhelder, B., and Piaget, J. (1964). The early growth of logic in the child. New York: Norton.

Jackendoff, R. (1983). Semantics and cognition. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

James, W. (1890/1983). The principles of psychology. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Jaswal, V. K., and Markman, E. M. (2002). Effects of non-intuitive labels on toddlers' inferences. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the International Conference on Infant Studies.

Jayaratne, T. (2001). National sample of adults' beliefs about genetic bases to race and gender. Unpublished raw data, University of Michigan.

Johannsen, G. (1973). Visual perception of biological motion and a model for its analysis. Perception and Psychophysics, 14, 201–211.

Johnson, A., and Ames, E. (1994). The influence of gender labelling on preschoolers' gender constancy judgements. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 12, 241–249.

Johnson, C., and Keil, F. (2000). Explanatory knowledge and conceptual combination. In F. C. Keil and R. A. Wilson (Eds.), Explanation and cognition (pp. 327–359). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Johnson, C. N. (1990). If you had my brain, where would I be? Children's understanding of the brain and identity. Child Development, 61, 962–972.

Johnson, C. N., and Jacobs, M. G. (2001, April). Enchanted objects: How positive connections transform thinking about the very nature of things. Poster presented at the meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Minneapolis, MN.

Johnson, K. E., and Eilers, A. T. (1998). Effects of knowledge and development on subordinate level categorization. Cognitive Development, 13, 515–545.

Johnson, K., Mervis, C., and Boster, J. (1992). Developmental changes within the structure of the mammal domain. Developmental Psychology, 28, 74–83.

Johnson, S. C., and Carey, S. (1998). Knowledge enrichment and conceptual change in folkbiology: Evidence from Williams syndrome. Cognitive Psychology, 37, 156–200.

(p.351) Johnson, S. C., Slaughter, V., and Carey, S. (1998). Whose gaze will infants follow? The elicitation of gaze following in 12–month–olds. Developmental Science, 1, 233–238.

Johnson, S. C., and Solomon, G. E. A. (1997). Why dogs have puppies and cats have kittens: The role of birth in young children's understanding of biological origins. Child Development, 68, 404–419.

Johnson–Laird, P. N., and Byrne, R. M. J. (1991). Deduction. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Johnson–Laird, P. N., and Wason, P. C. (Eds.). (1977). Thinking: Readings in cognitive science. New York: Cambridge University Press.

Jones, S., and Smith, L. (1993). The place of perception in children's concepts. Cognitive Development, 8, 113–39.

Jones, S. S., Smith, L. B., and Landau, B. (1991). Object properties and knowledge in early lexical learning. Child Development, 62, 499–516.

Josephson, J. R., and Josephson, S. G. (Eds.). (1994). Abductive inference: Computation, philosophy, technology. New York: Cambridge University Press.

Kahneman, D., Slovic, P., and Tversky, A. (Eds.). (1982). Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. New York: Cambridge University Press.

Kaiser, M. K., McCloskey, M., and Proffitt, D. R. (1986). Development of intuitive theories of motion: Curvilinear motion in the absence of external forces. Developmental Psychology, 22, 67–71.

Kalish, C. W. (1995). Graded membership in animal and artifact categories. Memory and Cognition, 23, 335–353.

Kalish, C. W. (1996). Preschoolers' understanding of germs as invisible mechanisms. Cognitive Development, 11, 83–106.

Kalish, C. W. (1998). Natural and artificial kinds: Are children realists or relativists about categories? Developmental Psychology, 34, 376–391.

Kalish, C. W. (2001). Children's inductive inferences about natural and intentional causal relations. Unpublished ms., University of Wisconsin–Madison.

Kalish, C. W. (2002). Gold, jade, and emeruby: The value of naturalness for theories of concepts and categories. Journal of Theoretical and Philosophical Psychology, 22, 45–56.

Kalish, C. W., and Gelman, S. A. (1992). On wooden pillows: Young children's understanding of category implications. Child Development, 63, 1536–1557.

Kamp, H., and Partee, B. (1995). Prototype theory and compositionality. Cognition, 57, 129–191.

Kanouse, D. E. (1987). Language, labeling, and attribution. In E. E. Jones (Ed.), Attribution: Perceiving the causes of behavior (pp. 121–135). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Kanouse, D. E., and Abelson, R. P. (1967). Language variables affecting the persuasiveness of simple communications. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 7, 158–163.

Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1977). More about the same: Children's understanding of post–articles. Journal of Child Language, 4, 377–394.

Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1979). A functional approach to child language: A study of determiners and reference. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1992). Beyond modularity: A developmental perspective on cognitive science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Karniol, R. (1980). A conceptual analysis of immanent justice responses in children. Child Development, 51, 118–130.

Katz, N., Baker, E., and Macnamara, J. (1974). What's in a name? A study of how children learn common and proper names. Child Development, 45, 469–473.

(p.352) Keil, F. C. (1979). Semantic and conceptual development: An ontological perspective. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Keil, F. C. (1987). Conceptual development and category structure. In U. Neisser (Ed.), Concepts and conceptual development: Ecological and intellectual factors in categorization (pp. 175–200). New York: Cambridge University Press.

Keil, F. (1989). Concepts, kinds, and cognitive development. Cambridge, MA: Bradford Book/MIT Press.

Keil, F. (1994). The birth and nurturance of concepts by domains: The origins of concepts of living things. In L. A. Hirschfeld and S. A. Gelman (Eds.), Mapping the mind: Domain specificity in cognition and culture (pp. 234–254). New York: Cambridge University Press.

Keil, F. C. (1995). The growth of causal understandings of natural kinds. In D. Sperber, D. Premack, and A. Premack (Eds.), Causal cognition: A multidisciplinary debate (pp. 234–262). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Keil, F. C. (1998). Words, moms, and things: Language as a road map to reality. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 63, 149–157.

Keil, F. C., and Richardson, D. C. (1999). Species, stuff, and patterns of causation. In R. A. Wilson (Ed.), Species: New interdisciplinary essays. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Keil, F. C., Smith, W. C., Simons, D. J., and Levin, D. T. (1998). Two dogmas of conceptual empiricism: Implications for hybrid models of the structure of knowledge. Cognition, 65, 103–135.

Kelemen, D. (1999a). The scope of teleological thinking in preschool children. Cognition, 70, 241–272.

Kelemen, D. (1999b). Why are rocks pointy? Children's preference for teleological explanations of the natural world. Developmental Psychology, 35, 1440–1452.

Kelley, H. H. (1967). Attribution theory in social psychology. Nebraska symposium on motivation, 15, 192–238.

Kemler Nelson, D. G. (1999). Attention to functional properties in toddlers' naming and problem–solving. Cognitive Development, 14, 77–100.

Kemler Nelson, D. G., and 11 Swarthmore College Students. (1995). Principle–based inferences in young children's categorization: Revisiting the impact of function on the naming of artifacts. Cognitive Development, 10, 347–380.

Kemler Nelson, D. G., Frankenfield, A., Morris, C., and Blair, E. (2000). Young children's use of functional information to categorize artifacts: Three factors that matter. Cognition, 77, 133–168.

Kemler Nelson, D. G., Russell, R., Duke, N., and Jones, K. (2000). Two–year–olds will name artifacts by their functions. Child Development, 71, 1271–1288.

King-Smith, D. (1983). Babe: The gallant pig. New York: Crown.

Kirkham, N. Z., Slemmer, J. A., and Johnson, S. P. (2002). Visual statistical learning in infancy: Evidence of a domain general learning mechanism. Cognition, 83, B35–B42.

Kister, M. C., and Patterson, C. J. (1980). Children's conceptions of the causes of illness: Understanding of contagion and use of immanent justice. Child Development, 51, 839–846.

Kohlberg, L. (1966). A cognitive–developmental analysis of children's sexrole concepts and attitudes. In E. Maccoby (Ed.), The development of sex differences (pp. 82–173). Palo Alto, CA: Stanford University Press.

(p.353) Kolstad, V., and Baillargeon, R. (1996). Appearance and knowledge–based responses of 10.5-month–old infants to containers. Unpublished ms., University of Illinois.

Kornblith, H. (1993). Inductive inference and its natural ground: An essay in naturalistic epistemology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Koslowski, B. (1996). Theory and evidence: The development of scientific reasoning. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Krascum, R. M., and Andrews, S. (1998). The effects of theories on children's acquisition of family–resemblance categories. Child Development, 69, 333–346.

Krifka, M. (1995). Common nouns: A contrastive analysis of Chinese and English. In G. N. Carlson and F. J. Pelletier (Eds.), The generic book (pp. 398–411). Chicago: Chicago University Press.

Krifka, M., Pelletier, F. J., Carlson, G. N., ter Meulen, A., Link, G., and Chierchia, G. (1995). Genericity: An introduction. In G. N. Carlson and F. J. Pelletier (Eds.), The generic book (pp. 1–124). Chicago: Chicago University Press.

Kripke, S. (1971). Identity and necessity. In M. K. Munitz (Ed.), Identity and individuation. New York: New York University Press.

Kripke, S. (1972). Naming and necessity. In D. Davidson and G. Harman (Eds.), Semantics of natural language. Dordrecht: D. Reidel.

Kuczaj, S. (1976). -Ing, -s and -ed: A study of the acquisition of certain verb inflections. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Minnesota.

Kuczaj, S. A. (1982). Young children's overextensions of object words in comprehension and/or production: Support for a prototype theory of early object word meaning. First Language, 3, 93–105.

Kuczaj, S. A. (1986). Thoughts on the intentional basis of early object word extension: Evidence from comprehension and production. In S. A. Kuczaj and M. D. Barrett (Eds.), The development of word meaning (pp. 99–120). New York: Springer–Verlag.

Kuhn, D. (1989). Children and adults as intuitive scientists. Psychological Review, 96, 674–689.

Kuhn, D. (1995). Microgenetic study of change: What has it told us? Psychological Science, 6, 133–139.

Kuzmak, S. D., and Gelman, R. (1986). Young children's understanding of random phenomena. Child Development, 57, 559–566.

Labov, W. (1973). The boundaries of words and their meanings. In C. N. Bailey and R. W. Shy (Eds.), New ways of analyzing variations in English (pp. 340–373). Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press.

Lakoff, G. (1987). Women, fire, and dangerous things. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Lamberts, K. (1995). Categorization under time pressure. Journal of Experimental Psychology:General, 124, 161–180.

Landau, B. (1982). Will the real grandmother please stand up? The psychological reality of dual meaning representations. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 11, 47–62.

Landau, B., Jones, S. S., and Smith, L. B (1992). Perception, ontology, and naming in young children: Commentary on Soja, Carey, and Spelke. Cognition, 43, 85–91.

Landau, B., Smith, L. B., and Jones, S. S. (1988). The importance of shape in early lexical learning. Cognitive Development, 3, 299–321.

Landau, B., Smith, L. B., and Jones, S. S. (1998). Object shape, object function, and object name. Journal of Memory and Language, 38, 1–27.

(p.354) Langer, E. J. (1975). The illusion of control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32, 311–328.

Langer, E., and Roth, S. (1975). Heads I win, tails it's chance: The illusion of control as a function of the sequence of outcomes in a purely chance task. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32, 951–955.

Lawler, J. M. (1973). Tracking the generic toad. Papers from the ninth regional meeting of the Chicago Linguistic Society (pp. 320–331). Chicago: Chicago Linguistic Society.

Leslie, A. M. (1994). ToMM, ToBY, and Agency: Core architecture and domain specificity. In L. A. Hirschfeld and S. A. Gelman (Eds.), Mapping the mind: Domain specificity in cognition and culture (pp. 119–148). New York: Cambridge University Press.

Leslie, A. M., and Keeble, S. (1987). Do six–month–old infants perceive causality? Cognition, 25, 265–288.

Levinson, S. C. (1996). Language and space. Annual Review of Anthropology, 25, 353–382.

Lévi-Strauss, C. (1963). Totemism. Boston: Beacon Press.

Levy, G., Taylor, M. G., and Gelman, S. A. (1995). Traditional and evaluative aspects of flexibility in gender roles, social conventions, moral rules, and physical laws. Child Development, 66, 515–531.

Li, P., and Bowerman, M. (1998). The acquisition of lexical and grammatical aspect in Chinese. First Language, 18, 311–350.

Liben, L. S., and Signorella, M. L. (Eds.). (1987). Children's gender schemata. (New Directions for Child Development, No. 38.) San Francisco: Jossey–Bass.

Lieven, E. V. M. (1994). Crosslinguistic and crosscultural aspects of language addressed to children. In C. Gallaway and B. J. Richards (Eds.), Input and interaction in language acquisition (pp. 56–73). Cambridge University Press.

Liittschwager, J. C., and Markman, E. M. (1993). Young children's understanding of proper nouns versus common nouns. Poster presented at the biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, New Orleans, LA.

Lillard, A. (1999). Developing a cultural theory of mind: The CIAO approach. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 8, 57–61.

Litt, C. J. (1986). Theories of transitional object attachment: An overview. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 9, 383–399.

Livesley, W. J., and Bromley, B. D. (1973). Person perception in childhood and adolescence. New York: Basic.

Lizotte, D. J., and Gelman, S. A. (1999). Essentialism in children's categories. Poster presented at the Cognitive Development Society, Chapel Hill, NC.

Lo, Y., Sides, A., Rozelle, J., and Osherson, D. (2002). Evidential diversity and premise probability in young children's inductive judgment. Cognitive Science, 26, 181–206.

Locke, J. (1959). An essay concerning human understanding, vol. 2. New York: Dover. (Originally published in 1671.)

López, A., Gelman, S. A., Gutheil, G., and Smith, E. E. (1992). The development of category–based induction. Child Development, 63, 1070–1090.

Lucariello, J., and Nelson, K. (1986). Context effects on lexical specificity in maternal and child discourse. Journal of Child Language, 13, 507–522.

Lucy, J. (1992). Language diversity and thought: A reformulation of the linguistic relativity hypothesis. New York: Cambridge University Press.

Lucy, J. (1997). Linguistic relativity. Annual Review of Anthropology, 26, 291–312.

Lugaresi, E., Gambetti, P., and Rossi, P. (1966). Chronic neurogenic muscular atrophies of (p.355) infancy: Their nosological relationships with Werdnig-Hoffman's disease. Journal of Neurological Science, 3, 399–407.

Lynch, E., Coley, J., and Medin, D. (2000). Tall is typical: Central tendency, ideal dimensions and graded category structure among tree experts and novices. Memory and Cognition, 28, 41–50.

Lyons, J. (1977). Semantics, Vol. 1. New York: Cambridge University Press.

Macario, J. F., Shipley, E. F., and Billman, D. O. (1990). Induction from a single instance: Formation of a novel category. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 50, 179–199.

Macnamara, J. (1982). Names for things. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press/Bradford Book.

Macnamara, J. (1986). A border dispute. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

MacWhinney, B. (1987). The competition model. In B. MacWhinney (Ed.), Mechanisms of language acquisition (pp. 249–308). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

MacWhinney, B. (1991). The CHILDES project: Tools for analyzing talk. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

MacWhinney, B., and Snow, C. (1985). The child language data exchange system. Journal of Child Language, 12, 271–296.

MacWhinney, B., and Snow, C. (1990). The child language data exchange system: An update. Journal of Child Language, 17, 457–472.

Mahalingam, R. (1998). Essentialism, power, and representation of caste: A developmental study. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Pittsburgh.

Mahalingam, R., Philip, C., and Akiyama, M. (2001, July). Expertise, essentialism and folk theories about the genetic causes of social differences: An expert-novice study. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of International Society for Political Psychology, Cuernavaca, Mexico.

Maloney, L. T., and Gelman, S. A. (1987). Measuring the influence of context: The interpretation of dimensional objectives. Language and Cognitive Processes, 2, 205–215.

Malt, B. C. (1990). Features and beliefs in the mental representation of categories. Journal of Memory and Language, 29, 289–315.

Malt, B. C. (1994). Water is not H2O. Cognitive Psychology, 27, 41–70.

Malt, B. C., and Johnson, E. C. (1998). Artifact category membership and the intentionalhistorical theory. Cognition, 66, 79–85.

Malt, B. C., and Smith, E. E. (1984). Correlated properties in natural categories. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 23, 250–269.

Mandler, J. M. (1988). How to build a baby: On the development of an accessible representational system. Cognitive Development, 3, 113–136.

Mandler, J. M. (1992). How to build a baby: II. Conceptual primitives. Psychological Review, 99, 587–604.

Mandler, J. M. (1993). On concepts. Cognitive Development, 8, 141–148.

Mandler, J. M., Bauer, P. J., and McDonough, L. (1991). Separating the sheep from the goats: Differentiating global categories. Cognitive Psychology, 23, 263–298.

Mandler, J. M., and McDonough, L. (1993). Concept formation in infancy. Cognitive Development, 8, 291–318.

Mandler, J. M., and McDonough, L. (1996). Drinking and driving don't mix: Inductive generalization in infancy. Cognition, 59, 307–335.

Mandler, J. M., and McDonough, L. (1998). Studies in inductive inference in infancy. Cognitive Psychology, 37, 60–96.

(p.356) Mangelsdorf, S. C. (1992). Developmental changes in infant-stranger interaction. Infant Behavior and Development, 15, 191–208.

Maratsos, M. (1974). Preschool children's use of definite and indefinite articles. Child Development, 45, 446–455.

Maratsos, M. P. (1976). The use of definite and indefinite reference in young children. New York: Cambridge University Press.

Maratsos, M., and Challdey, M. A. (1980). The internal language of children's syntax. In K. Nelson (Ed.), Children's language (Vol. 2, pp. 127–213). New York: Gardner Press.

Marcus, G. F. (2000). Pabiku and Ga Ti Ga: Two mechanisms infants use to learn about the world. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 9, 145–147.

Marcus, G. F., Pinker, S., Ullman, M., Hollander, M., Rosen, T. J., and Xu, F. (1992). Overgeneralization in language acquisition. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 57 (serial no. 228), 1–182.

Markman, E. M. (1976). Children's difficulty with word-referent differentiation. Child Development, 47, 742–749.

Markman, E. M. (1989). Categorization and naming in children: Problems in induction. Cambridge: Bradford Book/MIT Press.

Markman, E. M. (1992). Constraints on word learning: Speculations about their nature, origins, and domain specificity. In M. A. Gunnar and M. Maratsos (Eds.), Modularity and constraints in language and cognition (pp. 59–101). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Markman, E. M., and Callanan, M. A. (1983). An analysis of hierarchical classification. In R. Sternberg (Ed.), Advances in the psychology of human intelligence (Vol. 2). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Markman, E. M., Cox, B., and Machida, S. (1981). The standard object sorting task as a measure of conceptual organization. Developmental Psychology, 17, 115–117.

Markman, E. M., and Hutchinson, J. E. (1984). Children's sensitivity to constraints on word meaning: Taxonomic versus thematic relations. Cognitive Psychology, 16, 1–27.

Markson, L., and Bloom, P. (1997). Evidence against a dedicated system for word learning in children. Nature, 385, 813–815.

Marler, P. (1991). The instinct to learn. In S. Carey and R. Gelman (Eds.), The epigenesis of mind: Essays on biology and cognition (pp. 37–66). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Marr, D. (1982). Vision. New York: W. H. Freeman.

Martin, C. L. (1989). Children's use of gender related information in making social judgments. Developmental Psychology, 25, 80–88.

Martin, C. L., and Parker, S. (1995). Folk theories about sex and race differences. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, 45–57.

Martinez, I. M., and Shatz, M. (1996). Linguistic influences on categorization in preschool children: A crosslinguistic study. Journal of Child Language, 23, 529–545.

Massey, C., and Gelman, R. (1988). Preschoolers' ability to decide whether a photographed unfamiliar object can move itself. Developmental Psychology, 24, 307–317.

Mayr, E. (1982). The growth of biological thought. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Mayr, E. (1988). Toward a new philosophy of biology: Observations of an evolutionist. Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press of Harvard University Press.

Mayr, E. (1991). One long argument: Charles Darwin and the genesis of modern evolutionary thought. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

McCarrell, N., and Callanan, M. (1995). Form-function correspondences in children's inference. Child Development, 66, 532–546.

(p.357) McCawley, J. D. (1981). Everything that linguists have always wanted to know about logic. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

McCloskey, D. N. (1999). Crossing: A memoir. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

McCloskey, M. (1983). Intuitive physics. Scientific American, 248, 122–130.

McCloskey, M. E., and Glucksberg, S. (1978). Natural categories: Well defined or fuzzy sets? Memory and Cognition, 6, 462–472.

McConnell, J. V. (1962). Memory transfer through cannibalism in planarians. Journal of Neuropsychiatry (Suppl.), 42–48.

McDonald, K. A. (1998, October 30). Genetically speaking, race doesn't exist. Chronicle of Higher Education, p. A19.

McDonough, L., and Mandler, J. M. (1998). Inductive generalization in 9- and 11-montholds. Developmental Science, 1, 227–232.

McGarrigle, J., and Donaldson, M. (1974). Conservation accidents. Cognition, 3, 341–350.

McNamara, T. P., and Sternberg, R. J. (1983). Mental models of word meaning. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 22, 449–474.

McNaught, H. (1978). The truck book. New York: Random House

Medin, D. (1989). Concepts and conceptual structure. American Psychologist, 44, 1469–81.

Medin, D. L., and Ortony, A. (1989). Psychological essentialism. In S. Vosniadou and A. Ortony (Eds.), Similarity and analogical reasoning (pp. 179–195). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Medin, D. L., and Shoben, E. J. (1988). Context and structure in conceptual combination. Cognitive Psychology, 20, 158–190.

Medin, D. L., Wattenmaker, W. D., and Hampson, S. E. (1987). Family resemblance, conceptual cohesiveness, and category construction. Cognitive Psychology, 19, 242–279.

Mehler, J., and Fox, R. (1985). Neonate cognition: Beyond the blooming, buzzing confusion. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum Associates.

Merriman, W. E., and Bowman, L. L. (1989). The mutual exclusivity bias in children's word learning. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, Vol. 54, No. 3–4 (Serial No. 220).

Mervis, C. B., Johnson, K. E., and Scott, P. (1993). Perceptual knowledge, conceptual knowledge, and expertise: Comment on Jones and Smith. Cognitive Development, 8, 149–155.

Mervis, C. B., and Mervis, C. A. (1988). Role of adult input in young children's category evolution. I. An observational study. Journal of Child Language, 15, 257–272.

Mervis, C. B., and Pani, J. R. (1980). Acquisition of basic object categories. Cognitive Psychology, 12, 496–522.

Mervis, C. B., and Rosch, E. (1981). Categorization of natural objects. In M. R. Rosenzweig and L. W. Porter (Eds.), Annual review of psychology, vol. 32. Palo Alto, CA: Annual Reviews.

Metz, K. E. (1998). Emergent understanding and attribution of randomness: Comparative analysis of the reasoning of primary grade children and undergraduates. Cognition and Instruction, 16, 285–365.

Michotte, A. (1963). The perception of causality. New York: Basic Books.

Mill, J. S. (1843). A system of logic, ratiocinative and inductive. London: Longmans.

Miller, J. L., and Bartsch, K. (1997). The development of biological explanation: Are children vitalists? Developmental Psychology, 33, 156–164.

Miller, P. H., and Aloise, P. A. (1989). Young children's understanding of the psychological causes of behavior: A review. Child Development, 60, 257–285.

(p.358) Moravcsik, J. (1994). Essences, powers, and generic propositions. In T. Scaltsas, D. Charles, and M. Gill (Eds.), Unity, identity, and explanation in Aristotle's Metaphysics (pp. 229–244). Oxford: Blackwell.

Morford, J. P., and Goldin-Meadow, S. (1997). From here to there and now to then: The development of displaced reference in homesign and English. Child Development, 68, 420–435.

Morison, P., and Gardner, H. (1978). Dragons and dinosaurs: The child's capacity to differentiate fantasy from reality. Child Development, 49, 642–648.

Morris, M. W., Nisbett, R. E., and Peng, K. (1995). Causality across domains and cultures. In D. Sperber, D. Premack, and A. J. Premack (Eds.) Causal cognition. New York: Oxford University Press.

Morris, S. C., Taplin, J. E., and Gelman, S. A. (2000). Vitalism in naive biological thinking. Developmental Psychology, 36, 582–595.

Moser, D. J. (1996). Abstract thinking and thought in ancient Chinese and early Greek. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor.

Murphy, G. L. (2002). The big book of concepts. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Murphy, G. L. (1993). Theories and concept formation. In I. Van Mechelen, J. Hampton, R. Michalski, and P. Theuns (Eds.), Categories and concepts: Theoretical views and inductive data analysis (pp. 173–200). New York: Academic Press.

Murphy, G. L., and Medin, D. L. (1985). The role of theories in conceptual coherence. Psychological Review, 92, 289–316.

Naigles, L., and Gelman, S. A. (1995). Overextensions in comprehension and production revisited: Preferential-looking in a study of “dog,” “cat,” and “cow.” Journal of Child Language, 22, 19–46.

Naigles, L. R., and Terrazas, P. (1998). Motion-verb generalizations in English and Spanish: Influences of language and syntax. Psychological Science, 9, 363–369.

Nave, A. (2000). Marriage and the maintenance of ethnic group boundaries: The case of Mauritius. Ethnic and Racial Studies, 23, 32952.

Neisser, U. (Ed.) (1987). Concepts and conceptual development: Ecological and intellectual factors in categorization. New York: Cambridge University Press.

Nelson, K. (1973). Structure and strategy in learning to talk. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 38 (1 and 2, Serial No. 149).

Nelson, K. (1974). Concept, word, and sentence: Interrelations in acquisition and development. Psychological Review, 81, 267–285.

Nelson, K., Rescorla, L., Gruendel, J., and Benedict, H. (1978). Early lexicons: What do they mean? Child Development, 49, 960–968.

Nemeroff, C., and Rozin, P. (1989). “You are what you eat”: Applying the demand-free “impressions” technique to an unacknowledged belief. Ethos, 17, 50–69.

Nisbett, R. E., Krantz, D. H., Jepson, C., and Kunda, Z. (1983). The use of statistical heuristics in everyday inductive reasoning. Psychological Review, 90, 339–363.

Nisbett, R. E., Peng, K., Choi, I., and Norenzayan, A. (2001). Culture and systems of thought: Holistic versus analytic cognition. Psychological Review, 108, 291–310.

Nisbett, R. E., and Ross, L. (1980). Human inference: Strategies and shortcomings of social judgment. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

Nosofsky, R. M. (1992). Exemplars, prototypes, and similarity rules. In W. K. Estes (Ed.), From learning theory to connectionist theory: Essays in honor of William K. Estes. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

(p.359) Nosofsky, R. M., Kruschke, J. K., and McKinley, S. C. (1992). Combining exemplar-based category representations and connectionist learning rules. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18, 211–233.

Oakes, L. M., and Cohen, L. B. (1990). Infant perception of a causal event. Cognitive Development, 5, 193–207.

Opfer, J. E. (2000). Developing a biological understanding of goal-directed action. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor.

Opfer, J. E., and Gelman, S. A. (2001). Children's and adults' models for predicting teleological action: The development of a biology-based model. Child Development, 72, 1367–1381.

Osherson, D., and Smith, E. E. (1997). On typicality and vagueness. Cognition, 64, 189–206.

Osherson, D. N., Smith, E. E., Wilkie, O., López, A., and Shafir, E. (1990). Category-based induction. Psychological Review, 97, 185–200.

Pappas, A., and Gelman, S. A. (1998). Generic noun phrases in mother-child conversations. Journal of Child Language, 25, 19–33.

Parker, D. S. (1998). The idea of the middle class: White-collar workers and Peruvian society,1900–1950. University Park, PA: Pennsylvania State University Press.

Parritz, R. H., Mangelsdorf, S., and Gunnar, M. R. (1992). Control, social referencing, and the infants' appraisal of threat. In S. Feinman (Ed.), Social referencing and the social construction of reality in infancy (pp. 209–228). New York: Plenum.

Passman, R. H. (1987). Attachments to inanimate objects: Are children who have security blankets insecure? Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 55, 825–830.

Peevers, B. H., and Secord, P. F. (1973). Developmental changes in attribution of descriptive concepts to persons. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 27, 120–128.

Petrovich, O. (1999). Preschool children's understanding of the dichotomy between the natural and the artificial. Psychological Reports, 84, 3–27.

Piaget, J. (1929). The child's conception of the world. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.

Piaget, J. (1930). The child's conception of physical causality. London: Kegan Paul.

Piaget, J. (1948). The moral judgment of the child. Glencoe, IL: Free Press.

Piaget, J. (1970). Piaget's theory. In P. H. Mussen (Ed.), Carmichael's manual of child psychology (Vol. 1). New York: Wiley.

Piaget, J., and Inhelder, B. (1975). The origin of the idea of chance in children. New York: Norton.

Pinker, S. (1994). The language instinct. New York: W. Morrow.

Pinker, S. (2002). The blank slate: The denial of human nature in modern intellectual life. New York: Viking/Penguin.

Popper, K. R., and Eccles, J. C. (1977). The self and its brain. New York: Springer.

Povinelli, D. J. (2000). Folk physics for apes: The chimpanzee's theory of how the world works. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Prasada, S. (2000). Acquiring generic knowledge. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 66–72.

Premack, D. (1986). Gavagai! Or the future history of the animal language controversy. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Putnam, H. (1970). Is semantics possible? In H. E. Kiefer and M. K. Munitz (Eds.), Language, belief, and metaphysics. State University of New York Press.

Putnam, H. (1973). Meaning and reference. Journal of Philosophy, 70, 699–711.

Putnam, H. (1975). The meaning of “meaning.” In H. Putnam, Mind, language, and reality (pp. 215–271). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

(p.360) Quine, W. V. (1960). Word and object. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Quine, W. V. (1977). Natural kinds. In S. P. Schwartz (Ed.), Naming, necessity, and natural kinds (pp. 155–175). Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press.

Quinn, P. C., and Eimas, P. D. (1996). Perceptual organization and categorization in young infants. In C. Rovee-Collier and L. P. Lipsitt (Eds.), Advances in infancy research (pp. 1–36). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

Quinn, P. C., and Eimas, P. D. (1997). A reexamination of the perceptual-to-conceptual shift in mental representations. Review of General Psychology, 1, 171–187.

Rakison, D. H. (2000). When a rose is just a rose: The illusion of taxonomies in infant categorization. Infancy, 1, 77–90.

Rakison, D. H., and Butterworth, G. E. (1998). Infants' use of object parts in early categorization. Developmental Psychology, 34, 49–62.

Rakison, D. H., and Cohen, L. B. (1999). Infants' use of functional parts in basic-like categorization. Developmental Science, 2, 423–431.

Rakison, D. H., and Poulin-Dubois, D. (2001). The developmental origin of the animateinanimate distinction. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 209–228.

Ramsey, P. (1987). Young children's thinking about ethnic differences. In J. Phinney and M. Rotheram (Eds.), Children's ethnic socialization. New York: Sage.

Rescorla, L. A. (1980). Overextension in early language development. Journal of Child Language, 7, 321–335.

Reznek, L. (1987). The nature of disease. New York: Routledge and Kegan Paul.

Rholes, W. S., and Ruble, D. N. (1984). Children's understanding of dispositional characteristics of others. Child Development, 55, 550–560.

Rips, L. J. (1975). Induction about natural categories. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 14, 665–681.

Rips, L. J. (1989). Similarity, typicality, and categorization. In S. Vosniadou and A. Ortony (Eds.), Similarity and analogical reasoning. New York: Cambridge University Press.

Rips, L. J., and Collins, A. (1993). Categories and resemblance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 122, 468–486.

Robinson, W. P. (Ed.). (1996). Social groups and identities: Developing the legacy of Henri Tajfel. Boston: Butterworth-Heinemann.

Rosch, E. (1978). Principles of categorization. In E. Rosch and B. B. Lloyd (Eds.), Cognition and categorization (pp. 27–48). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Rosch, E., and Mervis, C. (1975). Family resemblances: Studies in the internal structure of natural categories. Cognitive Psychology, 8, 382–439.

Rosch, E., Mervis, C. B., Gray, W. D., Johnson, D. M., and Boyes-Braem, P. (1976). Basic objects in natural categories. Cognitive Psychology, 8, 382–439.

Rosen, A. B., and Rozin, P. (1993). Now you see it, now you don't: The preschool child's conception of invisible particles in the context of dissolving. Developmental Psychology, 29, 300–311.

Rosenberg, C. (1997). Banishing risk: Continuity and change in the moral management of disease. In A. Brandt and P. Rozin (Eds.), Morality and health (pp. 35–51). New York: Routledge.

Rosenblum, T., and Pinker, S. A. (1983). Word magic revisited: Monolingual and bilingual children's understanding of the word-object relationship. Child Development, 54, 773–780.

(p.361) Rosengren, K. S., and Braswell, G. (2001). Variability in children's reasoning. In H. W. Reese and R. Kail (Eds.), Advances in Child Development and Behavior, 28. Academic Press.

Rosengren, K., Gelman, S. A., Kalish, C., and McCormick, M. (1991). As time goes by: Children's early understanding of biological growth. Child Development, 62, 1302–1320.

Rotenberg, K. J. (1982). Development of character constancy of self and other. Child Development, 53, 505–515.

Rothbart, M., and Park, B. (in press). The mental representation of social categories: Category boundaries, entitativity, and stereotype change. In V. Yzerbyt, C. M. Judd, and O. Corneille (Eds.), The psychology of group perception: Contributions to the study of homogeneity, entitativity, and essentialism. New York: Psychology Press.

Rothbart, M., and Taylor, M. (1990). Category labels and social reality: Do we view social categories as natural kinds? In G. Semin and K. Fiedler (Eds.), Language and social cognition (pp. 11–36). London: Sage.

Rothbart, M., and Taylor, M. (2001). Comment on Gil-White, “Are ethnic groups biological ‘species’ to the human brain?” Current Anthropology, 42, 544–545.

Rovee-Collier, C. K. (1993). The capacity for long-term memory in infancy. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 2, 130–135.

Rozin, P., and Nemeroff, C. (1990). The laws of sympathetic magic: A psychological analysis of similarity and contagion. In J. Stigler, R. Shweder, and G. Herdt (Eds.), Cultural psychology: Essays on comparative human development. New York: Cambridge University Press.

Sabbagh, M. A., and Baldwin, D. A. (2001). Learning words from knowledgeable versus ignorant speakers: Links between preschoolers' theory of mind and semantic development. Child Development, 72, 1054–1070.

Sachs, J. (1983). Talking about the there and then: The emergence of displaced reference in parent-child discourse. In K. E. Nelson (Ed.), Children's language, Vol. 4. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Saffran, J. R., Aslin, R. N., and Newport, E. L. (1996). Statistical learning by 8-month-old infants. Science, 274, 1926–1928.

Saint-Exupéry, A. de (1943). Le petit prince. New York: Reynal and Hitchcock.

Samarapungavan, A., and Wiers, R. W. (1997). Children's thoughts on the origin of species: A study of explanatory coherence. Cognitive Science, 21, 147–177.

Savage-Rumbaugh, S., Shanker, S. G., and Taylor, T. J. (1998). Apes, language, and the human mind. New York: Oxford University Press.

Schieffelin, B. B., and Ochs, E. (1986). Language socialization. Annual Review of Anthropology, 15, 163–191.

Schlottmann, A. (2001). Perception versus knowledge of cause and effect in children: When seeing is believing. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 10, 111–115.

Schor, N., and Weed, E. (Eds.). (1994). Essential difference. Indiana University Press.

Schwartz, S. P. (1977). (Ed.). Naming, necessity, and natural kinds. Ithaca: Cornell University Press.

Schwartz, S. P. (1979). Natural kind terms. Cognition, 7, 301–315.

Sera, M. D. (1992). To be or to be: Use and acquisition of the Spanish copulas. Journal of Memory and Language, 31, 408–427.

Sera, M., Rettinger, E., and Castillo-Pintado, J. (1991). Developing definitions of objects and events in English and Spanish speakers. Cognitive Development, 6, 119–143.

(p.362) Seuss, Dr. [Theodore Geisel] (1940). Horton hatches the egg. New York: Random House.

Seuss, Dr. [Theodore Geisel] (1961). The sneetches and other stories. New York: Random House.

Shatz, M. (1991). Using cross-cultural research to inform us about the role of language in development: Comparisons of Japanese, Korean, and English, and of German, American English, and British English. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.), Cultural approaches to parenting (pp. 139–153). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Shatz, M. (1994). A toddler's life: Becoming a person. New York: Oxford University Press.

Shi, R., Werker, J. E., and Morgan, J. L. (1999). Newborn infants' sensitivity to perceptual cues to lexical and grammatical words. Cognition, 72, B11–B21.

Shipley, E. F. (1989). Two kinds of hierarchies: Class inclusion hierarchies and kind hierarchies. Genetic Epistemologist, 17, 31–39.

Shipley, E. F. (1993). Categories, hierarchies, and induction. In D. Medin (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 30, pp. 265–301). New York: Academic Press.

Shipley, E. F. (2000). Children's categorization of objects: The relevance of behavior, surface appearance, and insides. In B. Landau, J. Sabini, J. Jonides, and E. Newport (Eds.), Perception, cognition, and language: Essays in honor of Henry and Lila Gleitman (pp. 69–85). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Shoemaker, S. (1984). Personal identity—A materialist account. In S. Shoemaker and R. Swinburne (Eds.), Personal identity. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.

Shultz, T. R. (1982). Rules of causal attribution. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, Vol. 47, No. 1 (Serial No. 194).

Siegal, M., and Robinson, J. (1987). Order effects in children's gender-constancy responses. Developmental Psychology, 23, 283–286.

Siegal, M., and Share, D. L. (1990). Contamination sensitivity in young children. Developmental Psychology, 26, 455–458.

Siegler, R. S. (1994). Cognitive variability: A key to understanding cognitive development. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 3, 1–5.

Siegler, R. S. (1998). Children's thinking. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.

Siegler, R. S., and Crowley, K. (1991). The microgenetic method: A direct means for studying cognitive development. American Psychologist, 46, 606–620.

Siegler, R. S., and Ellis, S. (1996). Piaget on childhood. Psychological Science, 7, 211–215.

Sigel, I. E. (1953). Developmental trends in the abstraction ability of children. Child Development, 24, 131–144.

Sigel, I. E. (1978). The development of pictorial comprehension. In B. S. Randhawa and W. E. Coffman (Eds.), Visual learning, thinking, and communication (pp. 93–111). New York: Academic Press.

Signorella, M. L. (1987). Gender schemata: Individual differences and context effects. In L. S. Liben and M. L. Signorella (Eds.), Children's gender schemata (pp. 23–37). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

Silverstein, M. (1986). Cognitive implications of a referential hierarchy. In M. Hickmann (Ed.), Social and functional approaches to language and thought (pp. 125–164). New York: Academic Press.

Simons, D. J., and Keil, F. C. (1995). An abstract to concrete shift in the development of biological thought: The insides story. Cognition, 56, 129–163.

Skyrms, B. (1966). Choice and chance: An introduction to inductive logic. Belmont, CA: Dickenson.

(p.363) Slobin, D. I. (1981). The origins of grammatical encoding of events. In W. Deutsch (Ed.), The child's construction of language (pp. 185–199). London: Academic Press.

Sloman, S. A. (1993). Feature-based induction. Cognitive Psychology, 25, 231–280.

Smith, C. L. (1979). Children's understanding of natural language hierarchies. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 27, 437–458.

Smith, C. L. (1980). Quantifiers and question answering in young children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 30, 191–205.

Smith, C., Carey, S., and Wiser, M. (1985). On differentiation: A case study of the development of the concepts of size, weight, and density. Cognition, 21, 177–237.

Smith, E. E. (1989). Concepts and induction. In M. I. Posner (Ed.), Foundations of cognitive science (pp. 501–526). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Smith, E. E., and Medin, D. (1981). Categories and concepts. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

Smith, E. E., and Osherson, D. (1988). Compositionality and typicality. In S. Schiffer and S. Stelle (Eds.), Cognition and representation. Boulder, CO: Westview Press.

Smith, E. E., Patalano, A. L., and Jonides, J. (1998). Alternative strategies of categorization. Cognition, 65, 167–196.

Smith, J., and Russell, G. (1984). Why do males and females differ? Children's beliefs about sex differences. Sex Roles, 11, 1111–1120.

Smith, L. B. (1995). Self-organizing processes in learning to learn words: Development is not induction. In C. A. Nelson (Ed.), Basic and applied perspectives on learning, cognition, and development: The Minnesota Symposia on Child Psychology, Vol. 28 (pp. 1–32).

Smith, L. B. (2000). Learning how to learn words: An associative crane. In Becoming a word learner: A debate on lexical acquisition (pp. 51–80). New York: Oxford University Press.

Smith, L. B., and Heise, D. (1992). Perceptual similarity and conceptual structure. In B. Burns (Ed.), Percepts, concepts and categories (pp. 233–272). Amsterdam: North-Holland.

Smith, L. B., and Jones, S. S. (1993). Cognition without concepts. Cognitive Development, 8, 181–188.

Smith, L. B., Jones, S. S., and Landau, B. (1996). Naming in young children: A dumb attentional mechanism? Cognition, 60, 143–171.

Sober, E. (1994). From a biological point of view. NY: Cambridge University Press.

Soja, N. N., Carey, S., and Spelke, E. S. (1991). Ontological categories guide young children's inductions of word meaning: Object terms and substance terms. Cognition, 38, 179–211.

Soja, N. N., Carey, S., and Spelke, E. S. (1992). Perception, ontology, and word meaning. Cognition, 45, 101–107.

Solomon, G. E. A., and Johnson, S. C. (2000). Conceptual change in the classroom: Teaching young children to understand biological inheritance. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 18, 81–96.

Solomon, G. E. A., Johnson, S. C., Zaitchik, D., and Carey, S. (1996). Like father, like son: Young children's understanding of how and why offspring resemble their parents. Child Development, 67, 151–171.

Sorrentino, C. M. (2001). Children and adults represent proper names as referring to unique individuals. Developmental Science, 4, 399–407.

Spelke, E. S., Kestenbaum, R., Simons, D. J., and Wein, D. (1995). Spatio-temporal continuity, smoothness of motion, and object identity in infancy. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 13, 113–142.

(p.364) Sperber, D. (1975). Pourquoi les animaux parfaits, les hybrides et les monstres sont-ils bon à penser symboliquement? L'Homme, 15, 5–24.

Sperber, D. (1994). The modularity of thought and the epidemiology of representations. In L. A. Hirschfeld and S. A. Gelman (Eds.), Mapping the mind: Domain specificity in cognition and culture (pp. 39–67). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

Sperber, D. (1996). Explaining culture: A naturalistic approach. Oxford: Blackwell Publishers.

Springer, K. (1992). Children's awareness of the biological implications of kinship. Child Development, 63, 950–959.

Springer, K. (1995, April). The role of factual knowledge in a naive theory of biology. Paper presented at the meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Indianapolis.

Springer, K. (1996). Young children's understanding of a biological basis of parent-offspring relations. Child Development, 67, 2841–2856.

Springer, K., and Keil, F. C. (1989). On the development of biologically specific beliefs: The case of inheritance. Child Development, 60, 637–648.

Springer, K., and Keil, F. C. (1991). Early differentiation of causal mechanisms appropriate to biological and nonbiological kinds. Child Development, 62, 767–781.

Springer, K., and Ruckel, J. (1992). Early beliefs about the cause of illness: Evidence against immanent justice. Cognitive Development, 7, 429–443.

Steinem, G. (1964). Beatle with a future. Cosmopolitan. [reprinted in: E. Thomson and D. Gutman (Eds.). (1987). The Lennon companion (pp. 31–38). New York: Schirmer Books.]

Sternberg, R. J., Chawarski, M. C., and Allbritton, D. W. (1998). If you changed your name and appearance to those of Elvis Presley, who would you be? Historical features in categorization. American Journal of Psychology, 111, 327–351.

Stoler, A. (1995). Race and the education of desire: Foucault's history of sexuality and the colonial order of things. Duke University Press.

Strevens, M. (2000). The essentialist aspect of naive theories. Cognition, 74, 149–175.

Sylvia, C., and Novak, W. (1997). A change of heart. Boston: Little, Brown.

Tajfel, H. (1970). Experiments in intergroup discrimination. Scientific American, 223, 96–102.

Tardif, T. (1996). Nouns are not always learned before verbs: Evidence from Mandarin speakers' early vocabularies. Developmental Psychology, 32, 492–504.

Tardif, T. Z., Gelman, S. A., and Xu, F. (1999). Putting the “noun bias” in context: A comparison of Mandarin and English. Child Development, 70, 620–635.

Tardif, T., and Wellman, H. M. (2000). Acquisition of mental state language in Mandarin-and Cantonese-speaking children. Developmental Psychology, 36, 25–43.

Taylor, M. (1984). Seeing and believing: Children's understanding of the distinction between appearance and reality. Child Development, 55, 1710–1720.

Taylor, M. G. (1993). Children's beliefs about the biological and social origins of gender differences. Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor.

Taylor, M. G. (1996). The development of children's beliefs about social and biological aspects of gender differences. Child Development, 67, 1555–71.

Taylor, M. G. (1997, April). Boys will be boys, cows will be cows: Children's causal reasoning about human and animal development. Poster presented at the biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Washington, DC.

Taylor, M. G., and Gelman, S. A. (1993). Children's gender and age-based categorization in similarity and induction tasks. Social Development, 2, 104–121.

(p.365) Templeton, A. R. (1998). Human races: A genetic and evolutionary perspective. American Anthropologist, 100, 632–650.

Thagard, P. (2000). Explaining disease: Correlations, causes, and mechanisms. In F. C. Keil and R. A. Wilson (Eds.), Explanation and cognition (pp. 255–276). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Thomson, J. R., and Chapman, R. S. (1977). Who is “Daddy” revisited: The status of two-year-olds' over-extended words in use and comprehension. Journal of Child Language, 4, 359–375.

Thompson, E. P. (1963). The making of the English working class. New York: Pantheon Books.

Tomasello, M. (1999). The cultural origins of human cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Tomasello, M., and Akhtar, N. (2000). Five questions for any theory of word learning. In Becoming a word learner: A debate on lexical acquisition (pp. 179–186). New York: Oxford University Press.

Tversky, B., and Hemenway, K. (1984). Objects, parts, and categories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 113, 169–193.

Ullian, D. (1976). The development of conceptions of masculinity and femininity. In B. Lloyd and J. Archer (Eds.), Exploring sex differences. London: Academic Press.

Vendler, Z. (1967). Each and every, any and all. In Z. Vendler, Linguistics in philosophy. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press.

Vygotsky, L. S. (1934/1962). Thought and language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Walker, S. J. (1992). Supernatural beliefs, natural kinds, and conceptual structure. Memory and Cognition, 20, 655–662.

Ward, T. B. (1983). Response tempo and separable-integral responding: Evidence for an integral-to-separable processing sequence in visual perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 9, 103–112.

Waxman, S. R. (1991). Convergences between semantic and conceptual organization in the preschool years. In S. A. Gelman and J. P. Byrnes (Eds.), Perspectives on language and cognition: Interrelations in development (pp. 107–145). New York: Cambridge University Press.

Waxman, S. R. (1999). The dubbing ceremony revisited: Object naming and categorization in infancy and early childhood. In D. L. Medin and S. Atran (Eds.), Folkbiology (pp. 233–284). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Waxman, S. R. (in press). Links between object categorization and naming: Origins and emergence in human infants. In D. H. Rakison and L. M. Oakes (Eds.), Early category and concept development: Making sense of the blooming, buzzing confusion. New York: Oxford University Press.

Waxman, S. R., and Booth, A. E. (2000). Principles that are invoked in the acquisition of words, but not facts. Cognition, 77, B33–B43.

Waxman, S. R., and Booth, A. E. (2001). On the insufficiency of evidence for a domaingeneral account of word learning. Cognition, 78, 277–279.

Waxman, S. R., and Braig, B. (1996, April). Stars and starfish: How far can shape take us? Paper presented at the International Conference on Infant Studies, Providence, RI.

Waxman, S. R., and Hall, D. G. (1993). The development of a linkage between count nouns and object categories: Evidence from 16- to 21-month-old infants. Child Development, 64, 1224–1241.

(p.366) Waxman, S. R., and Hatch, T. (1992). Beyond the basics: Preschool children label objects flexibly at multiple hierarchical levels. Journal of Child Language, 19, 153–166.

Waxman, S. R., Lynch, E. B., Casey, K. L., and Baer, L. (1997). Setters and samoyeds: The emergence of subordinate level categories as a basis for inductive inference. Developmental Psychology, 33, 1074–1090.

Waxman, S. R., and Markow, D. B. (1995). Words as invitations to form categories: Evidence from 12- to 13-month-old infants. Cognitive Psychology, 29, 257–302.

Waxman, S. R., Senghas, A., and Benveniste, S. (1997). A cross-linguistic examination of the noun-category bias: Its existence and specificity in French- and Spanish-speaking preschool-aged children. Cognitive Psychology, 32, 183–218.

Waxman, S. R., Shipley, E. E., and Shepperson, B. (1991). Establishing new subcategories: The role of category labels and existing knowledge. Child Development, 62, 127–138.

Weissman, M. D., and Kalish, C. W. (1999). The inheritance of desired characteristics: Children's view of the role of intention in parent-offspring resemblance. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 73, 245–265.

Welder, A. N., and Graham, S. A. (2001). The influence of shape similarity and shared labels on infants' inductive inferences about nonobvious object properties. Child Development, 72, 1653–1673.

Wellman, H. M. (1990). The child's theory of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Wellman, H., and Gelman, S. A. (1988). Children's understanding of the nonobvious. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Advances in the psychology of human intelligence, Vol. 4 (pp. 99–135). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Wellman, H. M., and Gelman, S. A. (1992). Cognitive development: Foundational theories of core domains. Annual Review of Psychology, 43, 337–375.

Wellman, H. M., and Gelman, S. A. (1998). Knowledge acquisition. In D. Kuhn and R. Siegler (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology, 4th ed., Cognitive development (pp. 523–573). New York: Wiley.

Werner, H., and Kaplan, B. (1963). Symbol formation: An organismic-developmental approach to language and the expression of thought. New York: Wiley.

White, E., Elsom, B., and Prawat, R. (1978). Children's conceptions of death. Child Development, 49, 307–310.

White, E. B. (1945). Stuart Little. New York: Harper and Brothers.

White, P. A. (1989). A theory of causal processing. British Journal of Psychology, 80, 431–454.

White, P. A. (1995). The understanding of causation and the production of action: From infancy to adulthood. Hove, England: Erlbaum.

Whitehead, H. (1986). The varieties of fertility cults in New Guinea (Parts 1 and 2). American Ethnologist, 13, 80–99, 271–289.

Whorf, B. L. (1956). Language, thought, and reality. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Wierzbicka, A. (1994). The universality of taxonomic categorization and the indispensability of the concept “kind.” Rivista di Linguistica, 6, 347–364.

Wilcox, T., and Baillargeon, R. (1998). Object individuation in infancy: The use of featural information in reasoning about occlusion events. Cognitive Psychology, 37, 97–155.

Williamson, T. (1994). Vagueness. London: Routledge.

Wilson, R. A. (1999). Realism, essence, and kind: Resuscitating species essentialism? In R. A. Wilson (Ed.), Species: New interdisciplinary essays (pp. 187–207). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

(p.367) Winnicott, D. W. (1969). The child, the family, and the outside world. Baltimore: Penguin Books.

Wisniewski, E. J. (1995). Prior knowledge and functionally relevant features in concept learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, 449–468.

Wisniewski, E. J., and Medin, D. L. (1994). On the interaction of theory and data in concept learning. Cognitive Science, 18, 221–281.

Wittgenstein, L. (1953). Philosophical investigations. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.

Wolf, A. W., and Lozoff, B. (1989). Object attachment, thumbsucking, and the passage to sleep. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 28, 287–292.

Wood, C. C. (1976). Discriminability, response bias, and phoneme categories in discrimination of voice onset time. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 60, 1381–1389.

Woodward, A. L. (2000). Constraining the problem space in early word learning. In Becoming a word learner: A debate on lexical acquisition (pp. 81–114). New York: Oxford University Press.

Xu, F. (1999). Object individuation and object identity in infancy: The role of spatiotemporal information, object property information, and language. Acta Psychologica, 102, 113–136.

Xu, F. (in press). The development of object individuation in infancy. In J. Fagen and H. Haynes (Eds.), Progress in infancy research, Vol. 3. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Xu, F., and Carey, S. (1996). Infants' metaphysics: The case of numerical identity. Cognitive Psychology, 30, 111–153.

Yamauchi, T., and Markman, A. B. (1998). Category learning by inference and classification. Journal of Memory and Language, 39, 124–148.

Younger, B. (1990). Infants' detection of correlations among feature categories. Child Development, 61, 614–620.

Zadeh, L. A. (1982). A note on prototype theory and fuzzy sets. Cognition, 12, 291–297. (p.368)